Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Public Health 1/2020

Open Access 01.12.2020 | Research article

The relationship between household chaos and child, parent, and family outcomes: a systematic scoping review

verfasst von: Samantha Marsh, Rosie Dobson, Ralph Maddison

Erschienen in: BMC Public Health | Ausgabe 1/2020

Abstract

Background

Household chaos, represented by the level of disorganisation or environmental confusion in the home, has been associated with a range of adverse child and family outcomes. This review aims to (1) identify how household chaos is measured, (2) chart study details of household chaos literature, and (3) map the existing literature with respect to the relationship between household chaos and child, parent, and family outcomes. We expect that this review will highlight the need to consider the importance of household chaos in child well-being research, particularly in those families where children may be more vulnerable to the adverse effects of household chaos.

Methods

We searched five electronic databases (last updated September 1st 2018) in addition to Google Scholar, and identified publications via a 3-stage screening process, which was conducted by two researchers. Published studies were included if they investigated the association between household chaos and child, parent, or family outcomes. Research that investigated household chaos as a mediator or moderator, or that investigated how the relationship between household chaos and the outcome of interest was mediated or moderated, were also included.

Results

One hundred twelve studies in 111 publications were included. The majority were conducted in the United States (n = 71), and used either cross-sectional (n = 60) or longitudinal (n = 49) study designs. Outcomes of interest were categorised into seven categories: (1) cognitive and academic (n = 16), (2) socio-emotional and behavioural (n = 60), (3) communication (n = 6), (4) parenting, family, and household functioning (n = 21), (5) parent outcomes (n = 6), (6) hormone (n = 8), and (7) physical health and health behaviours (n = 19). There was consistent evidence for significant correlations between household chaos and adverse outcomes across all seven categories in diverse populations with respect to age, disease status, and socio-economic status (SES).

Conclusion

There is consistent evidence for associations between household chaos and a number of adverse child, parent, and family-level outcomes. Household chaos may also help describe variations in outcomes between low SES and child development.
Hinweise
A comment to this article is available online at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s12889-020-08680-y.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
TV
Television
SES
Socio-economic status
CHAOS
Confusion, Hubbub, and Order Scale
Household chaos represents the level of disorganisation or environmental confusion in the family home, and is characterised by high levels of background stimulation, lack of family routines, absence of predictability and structure in daily activities, and an overly fast pace of family life [1, 2]. Importantly, the construct of household chaos has been associated with a diverse range of adverse childhood outcomes, including poorer social-emotional functioning, cognitive development, academic achievement, and behavioural problems [39].
Household chaos has been linked with caregiver education, family income, and, perhaps not surprisingly, the number of people living in the household, whereby a lower level of caregiver education, lower family income, and a greater number of people in the home are associated with greater levels of chaos [10]. Despite this, the construct also been demonstrated to be distributed across socioeconomic status (SES) [10], and further, associations between household chaos and adverse child outcomes remain after controlling for SES [3, 11]. For example, one study showed that household chaos was associated with reduced cognitive ability and IQ in children, even after controlling for parent education/IQ, the home literacy environment, parental negativity, parental warmth, stressful events, and housing conditions [3]. Household chaos may therefore represent a unique risk factor for various adverse childhood outcomes, rather than simply reflecting residual confounding with, for example, SES [10].
In addition to the main effects of household chaos, the construct has also been shown to both mediate and moderate relationships between known child risk factors and adverse outcomes. For example, one study documented that the relationship between household chaos and maternal executive function was moderated by SES, suggesting that the adverse effects of household chaos may be exacerbated in socioeconomically distressed contexts [12]. Other studies have also shown that household chaos may mediate relationships between child behavioural problems and bedtime resistance [13], and poverty and socioemotional adjustment [14].
Given the varied ways in which household chaos is associated with adverse child outcomes, it is not surprising that there appears to be growing interest in the construct. Yet despite this interest, and a seemingly large body of evidence demonstrating links between household chaos and a range of adverse child outcomes, no review has been conducted in this field to date. To this end, the goal of this study was to undertake a review to investigate the relationship between household chaos and child-, parent-, and family-level outcomes.
We decided that the ideal method of synthesising the knowledge base at this time, due to the disparate nature of outcomes assessed, age range and disease status of participants, frequency and duration of follow-up, and study designs used, was a systematic scoping review. The scoping review methodology allowed us to (1) investigate how household chaos is measured, (2) summarise the research on how household chaos is included as a primary risk factor of child, parent, and family outcomes, and (3) map the existing literature, with respect to relationships between household chaos and child, parent, and family outcomes. This enabled us to assess not only how household chaos is measured, which is necessary to ensure findings are generalisable across studies, but also what dimensions have been investigated. It also enabled us to summarise the extant scientific research without focussing on a specific outcome, research design, study population (e.g. disease population), or setting [15], therefore allowing us to make recommendations for future systematic reviews and meta-analysis within the field. This review seems timely given that there is also a need to better understand if effects are independent of other known risk factors, or instead reflect an important confounding factor.

Methods

Identification of studies

Published scoping review guidelines directed the conduct of this review [1620]. The protocol for this scoping review was not registered. MEDLINE, PubMed, Embase, PsycINFO, and Child Development & Adolescent Studies were searched from inception through to 1 June 2017, with updated searches run on 8 November 2017 and 1 September 2018. We also ran a general search of ‘household chaos’ in Google Scholar and assessed hits from the first 100 pages of results for eligibility. An iterative process was used to develop the search strategy. Words associated with potential child, parent, and family outcomes (e.g. attention, aggression, diet, sleep, literacy, parent-child interactions) were combined with words and concepts associated with household chaos (e.g. household chaos, family disorganisation), and words associated with our sample of interest (e.g. children, adolescents, mothers, fathers, family). These combinations were used to form search strings, which were applied and run in the different databases. Filters were used to limit the search to studies available in English. The reference lists of all included manuscripts were searched for any additional articles not identified in by the electronic search. Full details of the MEDLINE search strategy are provided in Table 1. The grey literature was not searched.
Table 1
Search strategy utilised for MEDLINE (from inception to September 2018)
Search
Search Term
Combination
1
Infant/ or Child/ or Adolescent/ or Mothers/ or Fathers/ or Parents/ or Family/ or child.mp. or infant.mp. or mother.mp. or father.mp. or parent.mp. or adolescent.mp. or teenager.mp. or children.mp.
 
2
(Family disorganization or Family disorganisation or hurried).mp. or (confusion adj2 hubbub).tw. or household disorganisation.mp. or household disorganization.mp. or (environmental chaos adj5 family).mp. or (environmental chaos adj5 home).mp. or (environmental chaos adj5 household).mp. or (environmental chaos adj5 house).mp. or (chaotic environment adj5 family).mp. or (chaotic environment adj5 home).mp. or (chaotic environment adj5 household).mp. or (chaotic environment adj5 house).mp. or (chaos adj5 family).mp. or (chaos adj5 home).mp. or (chaos adj5 household).mp. or (chaos adj5 house).mp. or (chaotic adj5 family).mp. or (chaotic adj5 home).mp. or (chaotic adj5 household).mp. or (chaotic adj5 house).mp.
 
3
(Child health or Obesity or Overweight or Sleep or Diet or nutrition or “screen use” or Television or “family meals” or eating or Self-Control or self-regulation or Anxiety or Stress or “effortful control” or Attention or aggression or Decision Making or Resilience or family functioning or Parenting or Family Conflict or parental attitudes or Parenting or academic achievement or reading or literacy or mathematics or language or cognition or socio-emotional or social-emotional).mp.
 
4
 
1 and 2 and 3
5
 
Limit 4 to (english language and humans)

Study selection

Publications identified through our search strategy went through three stages of screening. During the first stage, titles were screened to establish their relevance to the research question. If relevance could not be established from the title, the study was included in the next stage of screening. During the second stage, abstracts of the selected titles were reviewed according to our inclusion criteria. During the third step, full text articles of the abstracts selected during the previous stage were retrieved and further assessed for relevance. Two researchers conducted this process and disagreements were settled first by discussion and, if necessary, by consultation with a third researcher.

Inclusion criteria

We included any published research that investigated the association between measures of household chaos and child, parent, or family outcomes, including health, cognitive, and psychosocial outcomes, as well as family-level processes. We also included any published research that investigated household chaos as a mediator or moderator and any studies that investigated how the relationship between household chaos and the outcomes of interest were mediated or moderated.
Due to the scoping nature of this review, we included healthy children and parents, in addition to those with diagnosed medical conditions, developmental delays, and behavioural disorders. No limits were set on study design. We included any study that investigated the effects of the household chaos construct; including studies that used terms such as home chaos, environment chaos (restricted to the home), chaotic homes/households, hurried homes, family chaos, chaotic families, and chaotic living. In addition, given that household disorganisation is a defining feature of household chaos, we also included studies that investigated the effects of family disorganisation and household instability.
While lack of family routines and family meals may represent one dimension of the household chaos construct, alone they are not a proxy measure for household chaos. Recently it was shown that household chaos and family routines may actually represent two distinct constructs and, further, that lack of family routines represents a pathway through which household chaos adversely affects child outcomes [21]. For these reasons, studies focusing primarily on routines (and those specifically using the Family Routine Inventory) and/or frequency of family meals only were not included in the review.

Data extraction and synthesis

For each included trial, two authors extracted data using an extraction form designed and pre-tested for the purpose of this review. Final data extraction was completed in October 2018. Information extracted from each eligible study included: author(s), year of publication, manuscript title, name of cohort/study, study location, health status of child and/or parent, diagnosed conditions of child and/or parent, SES details, location details (i.e. rural or urban), study aim, whether household chaos was the primary focus of the study, study design, duration and number of follow-ups, whether the study investigated twins/parent-child dyads/individual child/family, number of children/families included, age range of included participants, ethnicity, how household chaos was measured, whether household chaos was reported in the results, outcomes of interest (specifically related to household chaos), whether household chaos was assessed as a mediator and/or moderator, whether the relationship between household chaos and the outcome was mediated and/or moderated, and study results.
Data were summarised to record the number of studies retrieved by country, year of publication, study type, and any characteristics of the population (e.g. low SES, disease status). Outcomes of interest were categorised into seven broad categories (Table 2): (1) cognitive and academic (2) socio-emotional and behavioural, (3) communication, (4) parenting, family, and household functioning, (5) parent outcomes (6) hormone, and (7) physical health and health behaviours. Studies could be included in multiple categories of outcomes if they reported outcomes that fell across different categories. A narrative review is provided for each outcome measure of interest.
Table 2
Specific outcomes by outcome category
Outcome Category
Specific Outcomes Assessed
Cognitive and academic
Academic achievement/performance
Cognitive ability/outcomes/development
Executive function
IQ
Study skills
Reading comprehension
Intellectual functioning
Socio-emotional and behavioural
Response to challenge and task persistence:
 • Response to academic challenge
 • Task persistence
 • Processing speed
Behavioural and social:
 • Behavioural (including peer problems, conduct problems, impulsivity, aggression, anger, and prosocial behaviour)
 • Anxiety and stress
 • Social-emotional (including social problems, social competence, adjustment issues, and negative emotionality)
 • Attention/ADHD
 • Risk taking (including substance use, sexual activity, violence-related risk-taking, delinquency, and cheating)
 • Self-regulation and inhibitory control
 • Depression
 • Temperament
 • Future beliefs and aspirations
 • Empathy
 • Callous-unemotional traits and psychopathic characteristics
Communication
Verbal/expressive vocabulary and non-verbal abilities
Receptive vocabulary
Parenting, family, and household functioning
Parenting:
 • Differential parenting
 • Parent-child closeness
 • Parenting – general
 • Parental self-efficacy
 • Parental mood
 • Harsh parenting
 • Discipline
 • Sibling relationships
 • Sleep-disturbing behaviours of family members
 • Parental reactions
 • Maternal attribution bias
 • Emotional availability
 • Child representation of family dysfunction
Household characteristics and food security:
 • Food security
 • Family meal atmosphere
Parent outcomes
Maternal executive function
Parent diet/eating behaviours/weight status
Maternal depression
Maternal fatigue
Maternal/parent sleep
Hormone
Cortisol levels/profile
Autonomic nervous system activity
Developmental stability of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity
Gut biomarkers
Inflammatory profile
Physical health, health behaviours, and communication disorders
Health/disease/disorder outcomes:
 • Child weight/weight status
 • HbA1c
 • Child health
 • Stuttering severity
 • Physical health symptoms
Diet and Dietary behaviours:
 • Eating in the absence of hunger
 • Maternal feeding goals
 • Food-related behaviours
Sleep:
 • Sleep
 • Sleep anxiety
Other health behaviours/outcomes:
 • TV viewing behaviours in a laboratory setting
 • Maternal perceptions about physical activity

Results

A total of 661 manuscripts were identified from searches of databases, Google Scholar, and reference lists, of which 218 were duplicates, leaving 443 that were screened for eligibility. Of these, 295 were removed at the title and abstract screening stages, leaving 148 for eligibility screening of full-text articles. After removal of 51 articles due to ineligibility, and inclusion of an additional 6 and 8 articles from the two updated searches in November 2017 and September 2018, respectively, 111 manuscripts (representing 112 studies) were considered eligible and included in the review (Fig. 1).
Studies were excluded for a number of reasons: (1) household chaos was included within a composite measure of family functioning or household environment, (2) no child, parent, or family outcomes were reported, (3) only predictors of household chaos were assessed, (4) the construct of household chaos was not adequately measured (i.e. a different construct was measured), (5) household chaos was only included as a covariate in the analysis and not reported in the results, (6) the paper reported simulation data, and (7) the paper was retracted.
A summary of characteristics of the included studies can be found in Table 3. Overall there was generally an even mix of cross-sectional and longitudinal study designs (n = 57 vs n = 52), in addition to two experimental/laboratory studies and one case-control study. With respect to the longitudinal studies, samples were primarily drawn from large, nationally representative, longitudinal cohorts, which was reflected in the relatively large number of analyses conducted in sample sizes of greater than 1000 participants (24/104; 23%).
Table 3
Summary of characteristics of included studies
Study Characteristic
 
Na
Type of study
Cross-sectional
60
Longitudinal
49
Experimental/laboratory
2
Case-control
1
Sample Size
< 100
16
100- < 1000
72
1000- < 10,000
22
≥10,000
2
Cohort/Study
TEDS
12
FLP
9
Head Start Cohort
6
MCS +/− NESS
5
Pakistan cohort
5
Western Reserve Reading Project
4
Stand-alone Head Start Cohorts
4
Florida Twin Project OR from the Florida State Twin registry
2
SECCYD
2
SIBS
2
Early Alliance Trial
1
NAPLAN
1
BIDS
1
Early Growth and Development Study
1
ESM
1
FACES, 2006
1
Food and Family Project
1
Fragile Families and Child Well-being Study
1
Guelph Family Health Study
1
Home Environment Comparison Study
1
PHDCN
1
Social and Educational Change (RISE) study
1
Wisconsin Twin Project
1
L-CID
1
SIESTA
1
Social and Character Development Research Program
1
Stand-alone studies/No study or cohort name
46
Context/Participants
Low income/rural
34
Twins
19
Adopted children
Child Diagnosis/Risk factor:
1
ADHD
2
Autism
1
Obesity
2
DRD4 risk
1
Stuttering
1
Sickle cell disease
1
Type 1 diabetes
2
Parent Diagnosis/Risk factor:
 
Depression/
3
Bipolar disorder
2
ADHD symptomology
4
Measure of Household Chaos Construct
CHAOS long form
44
CHAOS short form
42
CHAOS adapted
10
Other – questionnaire and/or direct observation
16
Studies were included multiple times, for example, if the study was conducted in more than one country, or a manuscript reported results from more than one study. Where data were taken from a longitudinal cohort but analysis was only conducted at one time point, studies were classified as cross-sectional. Where location of study was not reported, location of the first author was used, and where studies investigated participants as parent-child dyads, families, or twin pairs, the unit of participants was used for sample size reporting (e.g. 5000 twin pairs was reported as a sample size of 5000, and 75 parent-child dyads was reported as a sample size of 75)
TEDS Twins Early Development Study, FLP Family Life Project, MCS Millennium Cohort Study, NESS National Evaluation of Sure Start Impact Study, SECCYD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development, SIBS Sisters and Brothers Study, NAPLAN Australian Twin Study of the National Assessment Program - Literacy and Numeracy, BIDS Ben-Gurio University Infant Developmental Study, ESM Early Steps Multisite project, FACES 2006 Family and Child Experiences Survey, 2006 cohort, PHDCN Project on Human Development in Chicago Neighbourhoods, L-CID Leiden Consortium on Individual Development, SIESTA Study of Infant’s Emergent Sleep TrAjectories, ADHD Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, DRD4 Dopamine Receptor D4
aN = 112 studies reported in 111 manuscripts
There has been an increasing trend in recent years in the number of manuscripts investigating the relationship between household chaos and child/parent/family outcomes (Fig. 2), with the majority of publications from research conducted in the USA (n = 74) and the UK (n = 21) (Fig. 3).

Methods of household chaos assessment

Studies varied somewhat in the method of household chaos assessment, although overwhelmingly the most frequently used methods were the long-form version of the Confusion, Hubbub, and Order Scale (CHAOS) [22] (n = 44) and the short-form version of CHAOS [23] (n = 42). The long-form version consists of 15 questions in a True-False response format, with each item reflecting household characteristics that directly represent a chaotic home environment, while at the same time specifically excluding any items that represent adequacy of the dwelling or quality of furnishings, or imply unsanitary conditions. The questionnaire was developed to be used in families regardless of the child’s age, and has been demonstrated to have satisfactory internal consistency, test-rest reliability, and adequate psychometric properties [22]. The short-form version of CHAOS consists of six items rated on a five-point scale (1 = definitely untrue, 5 = definitely true), which has been demonstrated to have acceptable internal consistency [23]. Another 10 studies used adapted long- and short-versions of CHAOS, and the remaining 16 studies relied on questionnaires developed specifically for the study and/or direct observation in the home.

Outcomes of interest

A summary of study characteristics, including sample size, age of participants, study design, SES status of participants, measure of household chaos, and outcome/s of interest can be found in Table 4. Here we provide a brief narrative review of study findings, with outcomes of interest mapped into seven categories.
Table 4
Outcomes of interest
Outcome of interest
N
Twin
Agea
Designb
Low ses
Measure of HC
Outcomes Assessed
C/O
L
Cognitive & Academic [3, 7, 11, 2436]
 Asbury, 2006 [24]
2017
0 m, 4y, 7y
 
 
CHAOS short form
HC as moderator between non-shared environment variables and academic achievement
 Berry, 2016 [25]
1235
 
2 m, 7 m, 2y, 3y, 5y
 
Interview/ observation
Academic achievement; executive function
 Deater-Deckard, 2009 [3]
302
4-7y
 
CHAOS short form
IQ
 Garrett-Peters, 2016 [26]
1236
 
6, 15, 24, 36, 48, 58 m & kindergarten (USA)
 
Interview/ observation
HC as mediator between family income poverty and academic achievement in kindergarten
 Hanscombe, 2011 [27]
2337
9 & 12y
 
 
CHAOS long form
Academic performance
 Hart, 2007 [11]
287
Kindergarten or 1st grade, then 1st or 2nd grade (USA)
 
 
CHAOS short form
General cognitive ability; HC as mediator between shared environmental variance and cognitive ability
 Hur, 2015 [28]
444
 
36-70 m
  
CHAOS short form
HC as mediator between parent depressive symptoms and child cognitive outcomes
 Johnson, 2008 [7]
455
mean 6.1 years
   
CHAOS short form
Woodcock score
 Pike, 2006 [29]
5765
0-3y, then 4y
   
CHAOS short form
Cognitive development
 Shamama-tus-Sabah, 2010 [30]
203
 
8-11y
  
CHAOS long form
Academic achievement
 Shamama-tus-Sabah, 2011 [31]
101
 
8-11y
  
CHAOS long form
Learning at school
 Shamama-tus-Sabah, 2011 [33]
203
 
8-11y
  
CHAOS long form
Study skills
 Shamama-tus-Sabah, 2011 [32]
203
 
8-11y
  
CHAOS long form
Cognitive performance
 Taylor, 2014 [35]
 
7-13y
  
CHAOS short form
HC as mediator between respect for rules and reading comprehension
 Taylor, 2017 [34]
269
 
9-16y
  
CHAOS short form
Reading comprehension
 Yarboi, 2017 [36]
65
 
6-16y
  
Interview/ observation
Intellectual functioning; academic achievement; executive functioning
Socio-emotional & Behaviorual [3, 4, 6, 10, 14, 24, 25, 28, 29, 3133, 3783]
Response to Challenge & Task Persistence
  Brown, 2008 [42]
96
 
3-5y
 
Interview/ observation
Response to academic challenge
  Fuller-Rowell, 2015 [50]
256
 
9.2, 13.1, 17.5y
 
 
CHAOS adapted
HC as a moderator between lower SES and task persistence
  Tomalski, 2017 [72]
71
 
5.5 m
  
CHAOS long form
Processing speed
Behavioural & Social
  Asbury, 2003 [37]
4706
4y
  
CHAOS short form
HC as a mediator and moderator in the relationship between parenting and behavioural outcomes
  Asbury, 2006 [24]
2017
0 m, 4y, 7y
 
 
CHAOS short form
HC as moderator between non-shared environment variables and anxiety, hyperactivity, CP, and peer problems
  Berry, 2016 [25]
1235
 
2 m, 7 m, 2y, 3y, 5y
 
Interview/observation
Social problems
  Bobbitt, 2016 [38]
2447
 
3-4y, 3.5–4.5y (6 m follow-up)
 
Interview/observation
Behavioural problems; social skills
  Bridgett, 2013 [39]
85
 
4 m
 
CHAOS long form
HC as mediator in relationship between maternal self-regulation and infant negative emotionality
  Brieant, 2017 [40]
167
 
13-14y
 
 
CHAOS short form
HC as moderator in relationship between parental EF and impulsivity and adolescent EF
  Brown, 2010 [41]
123
 
4–7.5y
 
CHAOS long form
Child attention (assessed by both parent report and computer task)
  Calam, 2012 [43]
48
 
2-10y
  
CHAOS short form
Child emotional difficulties
  Chatterjee, 2015 [44]
929
 
Mean 16.4y
  
CHAOS short form
Substance use; sexual activity; and violence-related risk behaviours
  Chen, 2014 [45]
149
 
3-7y
  
CHAOS short form
Temperament-by-parenting interactionsd; HC as moderator
  Coldwell, 2006 [4]
118
 
4-8y
  
CHAOS short form
Child problem behaviour; HC as a moderator in relationship between parenting and child behaviour
  Deater-Deckard, 2009 [3]
302
4-7y
 
 
CHAOS short form
CP
  Dumas, 2005 [10] (study 1)
106
 
4-6y
  
CHAOS long form
Behaviour problems; social competence; anger; aggression; attention; ability to understand and respond to social cues
  Dumas, 2005 [10] (study 2)
676
 
7-11y
 
CHAOS long form
Behaviour problems (caregiver and teacher report)
  Evans, 2005 [14]
339
 
Mean 9.2y at baseline
 
CHAOS long form + 2 x questions on rituals and routines
HC as mediator in relationship between SES and socio-emotional adjustment
  Farbiash, 2014 [46]
84
 
4y at follow-up
 
 
CHAOS short form
HC as mediator in relationship between parental ADHD symptoms and preschool aggression
  Fisher, 2018 [80]
390
 
8y at baseline, 15y at follow-up
 
 
CHAOS long form (14 questions)
Psychopathic characteristics; CU; empathy; anxiety; aggression; delinquency; classroom climate quality as moderator of relationship between HC and adolescent outcomes
  Flouri, 2015 [48]
10,086
 
3y, 5y, 7y
 
 
3 questions from CHAOS + 2 questions about bedtime and mealtimes
Occupational aspirations at age 7 years
  Flouri, 2017 [47]
180
 
3y, 5y, 7y, 11y
 
 
3 x CHAOS questions
HC as mediator in relationship between low SES and CP in children with ADHD
  Fontaine, 2011 [49]
9578
 
4y, 7y, 12y
 
 
CHAOS short form
CP; CU traits
  Gould, 2017 [51]
520
6-15y
 
 
CHAOS short form
HC as moderator of relationship between genes and development of ADHD
  Gregory, 2005 [52]
6612
 
3y, 4y
 
 
CHAOS short form
Anxiety; HC as mediator of relationship between sleep problems and anxiety
  Gunzenhauser, 2017 [53]
263
 
Mean 8.59y
 
 
CHAOS long form
Self-regulation
  Hannigan, 2017 [54]
6646
9y, 12y, 14, 16y
 
 
CHAOS short form
Depressive symptoms
  Hardaway, 2012 [55]
731
 
Mean 3.4y
 
CHAOS long form
Inhibitory control; problem behaviour; aggression
  Human, 2016 [56]
261
 
13-16y at baseline (2-year follow-up)
 
 
CHAOS long form
Depressive symptoms; perceived stress
  Hur, 2015 [28]
444
 
36-70 m
  
CHAOS short form
HC as mediator between parent depressive symptoms and child socio-emotional development, and behavioural self-regulation
  Jaffee, 2012 [57]
6286
9y, 12y
 
 
CHAOS short form
CP; hyperactivity-inattention
  Kahn, 2016 [58]
115
 
Mean 13.97y
  
CHAOS short form
Moderated mediation analysis = Harsh parenting as a mediator in the relationship between parent CU traits and child CU traits, and then HC as a moderator of this relationship
  Kim-Spoon, 2017 [59]
167
 
13-14y
 
 
CHAOS short form
HC as moderator of relationship between parental control and social competence
  Lauharatanahirun, 2018 [81]
167
 
13–15 years
 
 
CHAOS short form
HC as moderator between parental knowledge and insular risk processing
  Laurent, 2014 [60]
200
 
9 m, 18 m, 27 m, 4.5y, 6y
 
 
CHAOS short form
Behaviour problems in adopted children
  Lemery-Chalfant, 2013 [61]
807
Mean 7.93y
  
CHAOS short form
Genes as mediator in relationship between HC and child temperament (passive gene-environment); HC as moderator in relationship between genes and child temperament
  Martin, 2012 [6]
1266
 
2.5y, 5y
 
 
Questionnaire designed for study
Attention; aggression; delayed gratification; Mediating role of maternal warmth and provision of learning materials in the home on the relationship between lack of routines and delayed gratification
  Midouhas, 2013 [62]
209
 
9 m, 3y, 5y, 7y
 
 
3 items from the CHAOS short form
CP in children with autism; HC as moderator of relationship between poverty and parenting, and psycopathology in children with autism
  Miller, 2017 [63]
380
 
Preschool (USA)
 
CHAOS long form
Negative liability; emotion regulation
  Mills-Koonce, 2016 [64]
~ 1292
 
2 m, 6 m, 15 m, 24 m, 36 m, first grade
 
Interview/observation
CU; CP; empathy; HC as moderator in relationship between parenting and child behaviours; Maternal sensitivity and harsh parenting as mediators in relationship between HC and child behaviour
  Oliver, 2008 [65]
285
8-10y
  
CHAOS short form
HC as moderator in relationship between within-pair differences in perceptions of the classroom environment and teacher report of less prosocial and more hyperactive behaviour, and greater conduct and peer problems
  Panico, 2014 [66]
6572
 
3y, 5y
 
 
3 items from CHAOS
Behavioural outcomes
  Pike, 2006 [29]
5765
 
0-3y, 4y
   
CHAOS short form
Behavioural outcomes
  Raver, 2015 [67]
1025
 
Followed between 6 m and 58 m
 
Interview/observation
Ability to recognise and modulate negative emotions
  Shamama-tus-Sabah, 2011 [31]
101
 
8-11y
  
CHAOS long form
Attention
  Shamama-tus-Sabah, 2011 [68]
203
 
8-11y
  
CHAOS long form
CP
  Shamama-tus-Sabah, 2011 [32]
203
 
8-11y
  
CHAOS long form
Internalising and externalising behaviours
  Shamama-tus-Sabah, 2011 [33]
101
 
8-11y
  
CHAOS long form
Adjustment problems
  Shapero, 2013 [69]
956
 
Followed between birth and 15y
 
 
CHAOS long form
Changes in emotional and behavioural problems between childhood and adolescence
  Shelleby, 2014 [70]
731
 
2-3y, 4-5y, 7.5–8.5y
 
CHAOS long form
HC as mediator in relationship between income and emotional problems and CP
  Supplee, 2007 [71]
120
 
2, 3, 4 & 5y
 
CHAOS long form
Externalising behaviours; Maternal monitoring as moderator in relationship between HC and externalising behaviours
  Towe-Goodman, 2011 [73]
636
 
Infant, toddler, 3y
 
CHAOS long form
HC as moderator in relationship between interparental aggression in toddlerhood and the development of child attention skills and early childhood behaviour problems at age 3 years, in low-income families
  Tucker, 2015 [74]
325
 
Mean 15.71y
  
CHAOS short form
Adolescents’ future beliefs; Mother’s hostility as moderator in relationship between HC and adolescents’ future beliefs
  Tucker, 2018 [82]
238
 
Followed between 10th grade (mean 15.59y) and 12th grade
 
CHAOS short form
Problematic substance use, depression; HC as moderator in relationship between hostile parenting and depression
  Valiente, 2007 [75]
188
 
7-12y
  
CHAOS short form
Effortful control; externalising behaviours
  Vernon-Feagans, 2016 [76]
1145
 
2, 6, 15, 24, 48, & 60 months
 
Interview/observation
Regulatory behaviours in kindergarten; Parenting and executive function as mediators of the relationship between HC and behaviours
  Vilsaint, 2013 [77]
364
 
2y, 5y
 
CHAOS long form
Internalising & externalising behaviours
  Vrijhof, 2018 [83]
201
34-66 m
  
CHAOS long from
Effortful control; cheating behaviour
  Wang, 2012 [78]
304
Mean 6.07y, 7.15y, & 8.30y
 
 
CHAOS short form
HC as moderator in the genetic link between attention regulation and externalising behaviours
  Wilkinson, 2013 [79]
~ 8800
2, 6, 15, 24, 48, & 60 months
 
 
CHAOS short form
HC as moderator in relationship between genes and depressive symptoms (Gene × Environment interaction)
Communication [6, 7, 23, 25, 84, 85]
 Asbury, 2005 [84]
2223
4y
  
CHAOS short form
Verbal and non-verbal abilities; HC as moderator in the heritability of verbal and non-verbal abilities
 Berry, 2016 [25]
1235
 
2 m, 7 m, 2y, 3y, 5y
 
Interview/observation
Receptive vocab
 Johnson, 2008 [7]
455
mean 6.1 years
   
CHAOS short form
Expressive vocab and phonological awareness
 Martin, 2012 [6]
1266
 
2.5y, 5y
 
 
Questions designed for study
Receptive vocab; Mediating role of provision of learning materials in the home on the relationship between lack of routines and receptive vocab
 Petrill, 2004 [23]
 
3 & 4y
 
 
CHAOS short form
Verbal and non-verbal abilities; HC as mediator in relationship between shared environment and verbal and non-verbal scores
 Vernon-Feagans, 2012 [85]
1145
 
2, 6, 15, 24, & 36 m
 
Interview/observation
Receptive and expressive language
Parenting, Family & Household Functioning [4, 10, 45, 75, 85101]
Parenting
  Atzaba-Poria, 2008 [86]
344
 
4-5y and an older sibling aged <=8y
  
CHAOS short form
Differential parenting
  Barnes, 2014 [87]
24,610
 
3y
  
3 questions from CHAOS
Parent/child conflict & closeness; home learning environment; parenting
  Chen, 2014 [45]
149
 
3-7y
  
CHAOS short form
Temperament-by-parenting interactionsd; HC as a moderator
  Coldwell, 2006 [4]
118
 
4-8y
  
CHAOS short form
Parenting
  Corapci, 2002 [88]
57
 
12-14 m
  
CHAOS long form
Parenting; Parental self-efficacy and parent negative mood as mediators in relationship between HC and parenting
  Deater-Deckard, 2012 [89]
147
 
3-7y
  
CHAOS short form
HC as a moderator of maternal EF, which itself was assessed as a moderator in the relationship between child CP and harsh parenting
  Dumas, 2005 [10] (study 1)
106
 
4-6y
  
CHAOS long form
Parental discipline, and accuracy and efficiency in a co-operative parent-child interactional task
  Kretschmer, 2009 [91]
118
 
4-8y
  
CHAOS short form
Sibling relationships; Parenting as a mediator and moderator in the relationship between HC and sibling relationships
  Mokrova, 2010 [92]
319
 
7y (group 1) & 10y (group 2)
  
CHAOS long form
HC as mediator and moderator in relationships between maternal and paternal ADHD symptoms and parenting
  Nelson, 2009 [93]
101
 
7y
  
CHAOS long form
Parental supportive and nonsupportive responses to child
  Pike, 2016 [94]
106
 
Siblings aged 4–8 years at baseline & 9–13 years at follow-up
 
 
CHAOS short form
Siblings’ perceptions of mothering and fathering
  Spilsbury, 2017 [96]
26
 
11-12y
d
  
CHAOS long form
Sleep-disturbing behaviours of family members
  Valiente, 2007 [75]
188
 
7-12y
  
CHAOS short form
Positive and negative reactions to child’s negative emotions
  Vernon-Feagans, 2012 [85]
1123
 
2, 6, 15, 24, & 36 m
 
Interview/ observation
Parenting as a mediator in the relationship between HC and language development
  Wang, 2013 [97]
160
 
3-7y
  
CHAOS short form
HC as a moderator in relationship between maternal attribution bias (e.g. whether they attribute child misbehaviour to the child’s intentions and dismiss situational factors) and parenting behaviour
  Whitesell, 2015 [98]
100
 
1, 3, 6, 9, & 12 m
 
 
DISCORD - an observer reported measure of HC
SES; life events; co-parenting; emotional availability at bedtime
  Wirth, 2017 [99]
84
 
7-13y
   
CHAOS long form
HC as mediator in relationship between children’s ADHD symptoms and inadequate parenting
  Zvara, 2014 [100]
962
 
2, 6, 15, 24, 60 months & 1st grade
 
Interview/ observation
Children’s representations of family dysfunction from drawings; mediating role of parenting in relationship between HC and family dysfunction
Household Characteristics and Food Security
  Fiese, 2016 [90]
221
 
Elementary school (USA)
 
CHAOS short form
Food security
  Martin-Biggers, 2018 [101]
550
 
2-5y
  
CHAOS short form
Family meals; food availability/security; family meal atmosphere
  Pinard, 2015 [95]
252
 
0-18y
 
CHAOS short form
Food security
Parent Outcomes [12, 101105]
 Deater-Deckard, 2012 [12]
153
 
33-88 me
  
CHAOS short form
Maternal EF; SES as moderator of relationship between HC and maternal EF
 MacRae, 2017 [102]
44
 
1 ½ - 5ye
  
CHAOS long form
Parent fat intake and cortisol levels
 Madigan, 2017 [103]
501
 
2-54 me
 
 
Adapted version of the HOME scale
Maternal depression trajectories
 Martin-Biggers, 2018 [101]
550
 
2-5y
  
CHAOS short form
Maternal eating behaviours; weight status; health status
 Thomas, 2016 [104]
42
 
32wkse
  
CHAOS long form
Maternal depression, fatigue, and sleep
 Whitesell, 2018 [105]
167
 
1, 3, 6, 9, 12 m
 
 
DISCORD
Parent sleep
Hormone [60, 106112]
 Blair, 2013 [106]
1292
 
2, 7, 24, & 36 mo, 5y
 
Interview/ observation
Cortisol and ANS activity
 Chen, 2010 [107]
50
 
9-18y
 
CHAOS long form
HC as mediator in the relationship between low SES and cortisol profiles
 Doom, 2018 [112]
242
 
35–62, 38–85, 84-122 m
 
CHAOS long form
Diurnal cortisol levels in response to social stress test
 Laurent, 2014 [60]
200
 
9 m, 18 m, 27 m, 4.5y, 6y
 
 
CHAOS short form
Developmental stability of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis activity
 Lumeng, 2014 [108]
331
 
3-4y
 
CHAOS long form
Morning cortisol levels
 Miller, 2013 [109]
34
 
3-4y
 
CHAOS long form
Gut biomarkers
 O’Brien, 2013 [110]
135
 
18-66y
  
CHAOS short form
Stress
 Schreier, 2014 [111]
244
 
13-16y
  
CHAOS long form
Inflammatory profile
Physical Health, Health Behaviours & Communication Disorders [13, 52, 82, 96, 101, 105, 108, 113124]
Health/Disease/Disorder Outcomes
  Appelhans, 2014 [113]
103
 
6-13y
 
CHAOS long form
HC as mediator in relationship between sleep duration and child weight
  Chae, 2016 [114]
125
 
Mean 12.6y
  
CHAOS long form
HbA1c
  Dush, 2013 [115]
3288
 
3y, 5y
 
Interview/ observation
Child health
  Khatiwada, 2018 [118]
401
 
Infants; followed during first 12 mo
 
 
CHAOS long form
Weight status
  Kraft, 2014 [117]
69
 
Mean 3.7y
  
CHAOS long form
Stuttering severity
  Levin, 2013 [116]
104
 
1-13y
 
 
CHAOS long form
HbA1c
  Lumeng, 2014 [108]
331
 
3-4y
 
CHAOS long form
Overweight
  Martin-Biggers, 2018 [101]
550
 
2-5y
  
CHAOS short form
Health status; weight status
  Tucker, 2018 [82]
238
 
Followed between 10th grade (mean at baseline 15.59y) and 12th grade
 
CHAOS short form
Physical health symptoms; HC as moderator in relationship between hostile parenting and physical health symptoms
Diet & Dietary Behaviours
  Asta, 2016 [119]
209
 
21, 27, & 33 m
 
CHAOS (not specified)
Eating in the absence of hunger
  Goulding, 2015 [120]
287
 
4-8y
 
CHAOS long form
Maternal feeding goals
  Martin-Biggers, 2018 [101]
550
 
2-5y
  
CHAOS short form
Food-related behaviours
Sleep
  Bartel, 2016 [121]
1402
 
12-19y
  
CHAOS short form
Sleep
  Billows, 2009 [122]
217
 
13-18y
  
CHAOS long form
Sleep
  Boles, 2017 [13]
72
 
Mean 4.7y
 
CHAOS long form
Sleep; HC as a mediator in relationship between behaviour and sleep outcomes.
  Gregory, 2005 [52]
6612
 
3y, 4y
 
 
CHAOS short form
Sleep; HC as mediator relationship between sleep problems and anxiety
  Spilsbury, 2017 [96]
26
 
11-12y
d
  
CHAOS long form
Sleep
  Whitesell, 2018 [105]
167
 
1, 3, 6, 9, 12 m
 
 
DISCORD
Sleep
Other Health Behaviours/Outcomes
  Brown, 2010 [123]
70
 
4–7.5y
 
CHAOS long form
TV viewing behaviours in a laboratory setting
  Pesch, 2015 [124]
278
 
4-8y
 
CHAOS long form
Maternal perceptions about physical activity.
C/O Cross-sectional/other, L Longitudinal, HC Household Chaos, EF Executive function, CP Conduct problems, CU Callous-unemotional traits, ANS Autonomic nervous system
aAge for cross-sectional/other study type presented as range (preferential; based on inclusion criteria), mean (if range not given), or age group (if specific age not provided; when age group is used the country where the study took place is also included, as definitions of age groups differ between countries); for longitudinal studies age at each follow-up was provided where available for range, mean, or age group
bIf multiple selected, then cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses conducted. CHAOS short- and long-form refer to the short-form [23] and long-form [22] versions of the Confusion, Hubbub, and Order Scale (CHAOS)
cTemperament-parenting-interactions = interactions between child temperament and parenting (i.e. maternal negativity and positivity)
dReported as longitudinal but conducted as a 14-week laboratory sleep study
eAge of the child

Cognitive and academic

Sixteen manuscripts were identified that investigated the relationship between household chaos and cognitive/academic outcomes [3, 7, 11, 2436]. The studies covered the spectrum of age groups, from very early childhood to later adolescence, although the majority were conducted in young children (i.e. ≤5 years). Outcomes assessed included executive function, IQ, general cognitive ability, and a range of academic measures, including reading comprehension, academic achievement, study skills, and learning. Overall, household chaos was consistently associated with adverse cognitive and academic outcomes. There was also evidence for an effect of household chaos on outcomes after controlling for SES [3, 30, 32]. However, null findings were reported in 2 of the 16 studies; one study (n = 203) did not find a significant relationship between household chaos and cognitive performance [33], while another study (n = 65) failed to demonstrate a significant relationship between household chaos and intelligence, academic achievement, and executive functioning in 6–16 years olds [36].

Socio-emotional and behavioural

The majority of studies investigated the relationship between household chaos and socio-emotional and behavioural outcomes [3, 4, 6, 10, 14, 24, 25, 28, 29, 3133, 3783], although 18/59 of these studies only investigated household chaos as a mediator or moderator in the relationship between a predictor and a socio-emotional/behaviour outcome. Outcomes included responses to challenges, social skills/competence, emotion regulation, risky behaviours, attention, aspirations, aggression, conduct problems, and callous-unemotional traits. Overwhelmingly, household chaos was shown to be associated with adverse outcomes in both younger children and adolescents [3, 4, 6, 10, 25, 3133, 38, 4144, 49, 55, 57, 6264, 67, 68, 72, 80, 82, 83]; however, it was not shown to be associated with self-regulation and effortful control [53, 83], empathy [64], sexual risk or other violent behaviours in adolescents [44], or occupational aspirations in 7 year olds [47].

Communication

A total of six manuscripts investigated the link between household chaos and communication, all of which were conducted in the early childhood setting [6, 7, 23, 25, 84, 85]. Outcomes assessed included non-verbal abilities, receptive and expressive language, and phonological awareness. Household chaos was consistently linked with adverse effects on communication outcomes across all six analyses. After controlling for all other measures of household chaos, lack of routines was significantly associated with lower receptive vocabulary scores in 5 year olds [6], and in an analysis that controlled for 13 covariates, including maternal education and poverty, household disorganisation was associated with significant decreases in both receptive and expressive language in 3-year-old children (n = 1145) [85]. Finally, when investigating the heritability of cognitive abilities as a function of the child’s early environment, household chaos, which is classified as a proximal environmental determinant, had stronger effects than distal environmental determinants (e.g. SES) on the heritability of verbal ability [84].

Parenting, and family and household functioning

Twenty-one studies investigated the effects of household chaos on parenting and family functioning [4, 10, 45, 75, 85101], of which five focused on the role of household chaos as a mediator or moderator. Outcomes assessed included parenting, parent-child interactions, discipline, sibling relationships, parental response and reactions to child behaviours, family dysfunction, and food insecurity. Household chaos was associated with increased parent-child conflict, decreased parent-child closeness, decreased supportive parenting, decreased positive parenting, and increased negative parenting [87], in addition to less favourable co-parenting, and less emotional availability at bedtime [98]. A chaotic home environment was also shown to be associated with less responsive and less stimulating parenting [88], less effective parental discipline [10], greater non-supportive responses to children’s emotions and fewer supportive responses [75, 93], and greater paternal hostility [94]. Greater chaos in the home was associated with increased odds of household members disturbing the efforts of adolescents to fall asleep, and decreased the odds of adolescents reporting that nothing was keeping them awake or making it difficult to sleep [96]. Finally, cumulative family disorganisation, but not cumulative family instability, was indirectly associated with children’s representation of family dysfunction in drawings, through parenting behaviours [100].
Food insecure households were more likely to have greater household chaos scores compared with food secure households, even after controlling for education and marital status, with higher chaos homes reporting less planning around mealtimes [90]. In another study, high and medium household chaos homes were more likely to experience low or very low food security compared with low chaos homes [95]. Further, low chaos predicted greater availability of fruits and vegetables in the home and more family meals, while high chaos was a significant predictor of food insecurity risk and greater availability of salty and fatty snacks [101].

Parent outcomes

The relationship between household chaos and parent outcomes, including maternal executive function, parent sleep, parent feeding behaviours and weight status, and maternal depression, were assessed in six studies [12, 101105], of which two studies included sample sizes of less than 50 participants [102, 104], and all of which were conducted in the early childhood setting. Household chaos was associated with poorer maternal executive function (n = 153), although the modest effect of household chaos overlapped with the effects of co-varying factors, including SES and verbal ability [12]. In parents of children aged 18 months to 5 years (n = 44), household chaos was significantly associated with fat intake and high serum cortisol levels; however, the relationship between household chaos and fat intake appeared to be somewhat mediated by cortisol levels, although this relationship was not significant [102]. In mothers of children aged 2–5 years (n = 550), high chaos was associated with greater engagement in emotional and disinhibited eating, while mothers in low chaos households were more likely to be adventurous eaters [101].
Household chaos was lower in mothers with trajectories of low-stable levels of depression compared with moderate-increasing levels of depression, but not in those with remitting depression [103]. In mothers of infants [104], household chaos was strongly correlated with measures of maternal depression, sleep, wake disturbances, and fatigue. Similarly, mothers and fathers of infants in high chaos homes demonstrated greater variability in sleep duration compared with low chaos families, while parental sleep fragmentation mirrored that of the child in low chaos homes, where fragmented sleep decreased for both the parents and child over the course of the first year [105].

Hormones

Eight studies investigated the role of household chaos in cortisol and autonomic nervous system activity, inflammatory profiles, and gut biomarkers [60, 106112], with sample sizes ranging from 32 to 1292 participants. For stress physiology, household chaos in early childhood was associated with (1) a blunted diurnal cortisol slope in middle school [112], (2) cortisol levels in 7 year olds who had lower levels of resting heart rate variability [106], (3) stable morning cortisol levels in 6 year olds [60], and (4) lowered morning cortisol levels in 3–4 year olds [108]. In 13–16 year olds [111], household chaos was associated with increased systemic inflammation, interleukin-6 (IL-6) levels, and C-reactive protein levels, although the relationship between chaos and systematic inflammation and IL-6 levels was moderated by SES. In participants aged 18–66 years, household chaos was not related with hair cortisol levels [110] or gut-derived biomarkers associated with appetite and regulation [109].

Physical health, health behaviours and communication disorders

Overall, 19 analyses were conducted that investigated the role of household chaos on health and health behaviours: (1) 9 papers specifically looked at physical health outcomes, disease, and communication disorder outcomes [82, 101, 108, 113118], including glycaemic control, child health, weight status, and stutter, (2) 3 studies investigated diet and dietary behaviours [101, 119, 120], (3) 6 studies looked at sleep [13, 52, 96, 105, 121, 122], and (4) 2 studies assessed other outcomes, including TV viewing behaviours in a laboratory setting [123] and mothers’ perceptions on children’s physical activity [124]. For glycaemic control, both maternal and paternal household chaos scores were positively associated with HbA1c in children and adolescents with type 1 diabetes [114] and in children aged 1–13 years with type 1 diabetes mellitus [116]. Chaotic homes were also linked with low cortisol levels (hypocorticolism) in children aged 3–4 years, which in turn predicted overweight in girls, both directly and indirectly through the mediating role of satiety responsiveness, and in boys, indirectly through the mediating role of emotional overeating [108]. In infants followed during the first year of life, household chaos was also found to significantly predict weight-for-height z-scores, even after controlling for possible confounders [118].
Chaos was found to be associated with maternal feeding goals [120], with lower household chaos associated with more positive maternal feeding goals, such as promotion of child autonomy around eating. Greater household chaos was also shown to be associated with greater consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages in preschoolers [101]. Further, chaos was associated with mother-report of general child health, even after controlling for SES, maternal health status, and family structure [115]. Similarly for older children, self-report of household chaos in 10-year-olds was associated with worse physical health 2 years later [82].
With respect to sleep, a more chaotic home environment was associated with mixed effects on sleep outcomes in adolescents, including sleep onset latency and sleep duration [121, 122]; however, in younger children greater scores for household chaos were significantly associated with higher parent-reported scores for bedtime resistance, sleep anxiety, and total sleep problems [13]. One study found that infants from highly chaotic homes demonstrated delays in sleep consolidation patterns and greater fragmentation of sleep; however, they also reported longer and more variable sleep duration compared with infants in low chaos homes [105]. The authors suggested that this unexpected difference in sleep duration may have reflected a higher quality of sleep in the low chaos households, where sleep was less fragmented and bedtimes and wake times less variable. Chaos in the home environment was not found to be associated with maternal perceptions about physical activity in children [124], stutter severity in children with stutter [117],TV viewing behaviours (i.e. looking patterns) in a laboratory setting [123], or eating in the absence of hunger in low-income toddlers [119].

Mediation and moderation analyses

Table 5 presents results from studies where mediation and moderation analyses were conducted. Household chaos was consistently found to mediate the relationship between predictors of adverse child outcomes. Importantly, a number of studies demonstrated the mediating role of chaotic homes between SES and outcomes [14, 26, 107], suggesting that the adverse effects of low SES on child outcomes may, at least in part, be mediated by the effects of household chaos. Further, chaotic environments were also shown to moderate the relationship between several predictors of adverse children and family outcomes, including SES [50], parenting [24, 45, 58], and parental executive function [40], whereby the effects of these predictors were more pronounced in highly chaotic homes. A number of studies also demonstrated the mediating role of parenting on the relationship between household chaos and adverse child outcomes (Table 6) [6, 76, 85, 91], where adverse parenting behaviours may partially explain the relationship between chaos and child outcomes.
Table 5
Household Chaos as mediator and/or moderator
Study
HC as mediator
HC as moderator
Asbury, 2003 [37]
HC did not mediate the relationship between parenting and behavioural outcomes.
HC significantly moderated the relationship between (1) differential harsh parental discipline and differential CP, and (2) differential negative parental feelings and anxiety, hyperactivity, and CP. That is, the relationship between NSE influences (i.e. differential parenting) and behavioural outcomes were stronger in high chaos homes.
Asbury, 2006 [24]
 
HC moderated the relationship between: (1) discordant harsh discipline and CP [increase in CP], (2) discordant negative feelings and CP [increase in CP], (3) discordant parental negative feelings and academic achievement [achievement decreased], and (4) instructive parent-child communication and anxiety [anxiety increased] (where instructive communication including things like correcting child’s grammar)
Boles, 2017 [13]
Children with higher scores for emotional and behaviour problems were more likely to have higher bedtime resistance, but higher levels of HC may account for this relationship; HC associated with bedtime resistance significantly mediated the relationship between Behavioural and Emotional Screen System and bedtime resistance.
 
Bridgett, 2013 [39]
HC mediated the effects of maternal self-regulation on infant frustration/distress to limitations.
 
Brieant, 2017 [40]
 
Parent EF significantly predicted changes over time in adolescent EF, with this association contingent on levels HC, such that a stronger relationship was observed in the presence of greater HC.
Chen, 2010 [107]
HC partially mediated the relationship between SES and daily cortisol output.
 
Chen, 2014 [45]
 
HC moderated the relationship between harsher maternal negativity and child maladjustment in children who had low levels of effortful control. HC did not moderate any other temperament-by-parenting interactions.
Coldwell, 2006 [4]
 
In a minority of cases, HC was found to moderate the role between parenting and child behaviour, whereby HC exacerbated the effect of poorer quality parenting on problem behaviour.
Deater-Deckard, 2012 [89]
 
Maternal EF moderated the relationship between child CP and harsh parenting, so that this relationship was only significant in mothers with poorer EF. Further, this moderating effect of EF was further moderated by HC, so that the effect was particularly strong in the presence of low HC, but not high HC.
Evans, 2005 [14]
The adverse effects of poverty on socioemotional adjustment were shown to be mediated by HC.
 
Farbiash, 2014 [46]
HC was found to significantly mediate the relationship between paternal (but not maternal) ADHD symptoms and children’s aggression in the preschool years.
 
Flouri, 2017 [47]
HC did not mediate the relationship between low SES and CP in children with ADHD
 
Fuller-Rowell, 2015 [50]
 
Early HC moderated relationship between poverty and task persistence in later adolescence.
Garrett-Peters, 2016 [26]
In the presence of household disorganisation (but not instability), income poverty was no longer directly related to academic achievement. Income was related to disorganisation, however, which in turn was associated with lower academic achievement.
 
Gould, 2017 [51]
 
HC did not moderate the relationship between genes and development of ADHD.
Gregory, 2005 [52]
HC partially mediated the relationship between sleep problems and child anxiety, with HC accounting for ~ 30% of the association.
 
Hart, 2007 [11]
HC mediated the relationship between shared environmental variance and longitudinal stability of cognitive ability in early childhood.
 
Hur, 2015 [28]
HC mediated relationship between parental depressive symptoms and mother-reported social skills of child.
 
Kahn, 2016 [58]
 
Hostile parenting mediated the relationship between parent and adolescent CU, but only in the presence of high HC. Harsh parenting explained intergenerational similarity in CU traits, and HC exacerbated this relationship.
Kim-Spoon, 2017 [59]
 
High parental control predicted better neural cognitive control in adolescents living in low HC contexts. Poor neural cognitive control was associated with reduced social competence 1-year later (after controlling for social competence at baseline), but this relationship was only significant in high HC contexts. HC was shown to undermine positive associations between parental control and adolescent neural cognitive control and exacerbated the negative association between poor neural cognitive control and social competence development.
Lauharatanahirun, 2018 [81]
 
HC moderated the relationship between adolescent-report of parental knowledge/parental monitoring and adolescent insular risk processing, which has been shown to precede risk-adverse choices, whereby it was only present in the presence of low HC.
Lemery-Chalfant, 2013 [61]
 
Heritability of children’s temperament was moderated by HC, meaning that effortful control and surgency were more heritable in high HC contexts.
Midouhas, 2013 [62]
 
Autism was associated with higher rates of psychopathology over time, and family poverty was associated with emotional and CP (psychopathology); however, home organisation did not moderate the relationship between family poverty and psychopathology.
Mills-Koonce, 2016 [64]
 
HC did not moderate the relationship between parenting and child behaviours.
Mokrova, 2010 [92]
Mother’s ADHD symptoms were positively associated with inconsistent discipline and non-supportive responses to child’s negative emotions; this relationship was mediated by HC. Father’s ADHD symptoms were associated with low involvement; HC mediated this relationship.
Father’s ADHD symptoms were associated with inconsistent discipline; this relationship was moderated by HC.
Oliver, 2008 [65]
 
HC moderated the relationship between within-pair differences in perceptions of the classroom environment and teacher report of less prosocial and more hyperactive behaviour, and greater conduct and peer problems. Specifically, associations between differential classroom environment and CP were greater for children in more chaotic homes.
Petrill, 2004 [23]
HC partially mediated an independent and significant portion of the shared environment for verbal and nonverbal and PARCA scores at ages 3 and 4 years. HC was a significant mediator even when controlling for SES.
 
Shelleby, 2014 [70]
Significant direct association between income and emotional problems and CP; HC was a significant mediator in the relationship between income and emotional problems.
 
Taylor, 2014 [35]
HC mediated relationship between respect for rules and increased reading comprehension, even after controlling for income.
 
Towe-Goodman, 2011 [73]
 
In a low-income sub-group, HC was found to moderate the relationship between interparental aggression in toddlerhood and the development of child attention skills and early childhood behaviour problems at age 3 years. In low-income families with low HC, there was a significant association between greater interparental aggression and increases 3-year ADHD symptoms, but this relationship was not significant in low-income, high HC families.
Tucker, 2018 [82]
 
HC moderated the effect of hostile parenting on adolescents’ depression 2 years later, so that depression was exacerbated in the context of high HC. HC did not moderate the relationship between parenting and physical health and problematic substance use.
Wang, 2012 [78]
 
HC moderated the genetic variance and covariance between externalising problems and attention regulation, with the genetic influences stronger in HC contexts; however, higher levels of HC attenuated the genetic association between externalising problems and attention regulation.
Wang, 2013 [97]
 
The relationship between maternal attribution bias and parenting behaviour were stronger in more chaotic environments; the moderating effect of HC was particularly strong for internal attribution bias.
Wilkinson, 2013 [79]
 
Depressive symptoms at age 12 were significantly heritable, and HC and parenting style at age 9 years moderated this association.
Wirth, 2017 [99]
The relationships between ADHD and positive parenting, corporal punishment, and inconsistent discipline were somewhat mediated by HC. As such, high HC was associated with specific parenting dimensions in families with children with ADHD.
 
NSE Non-shared environment
Table 6
Mediation and moderation analysis of relationship between household chaos and outcome
Study
Relationship between HC and outcome mediated
Relationship between HC and outcome moderated
Asbury, 2005 [84]
 
There was greater group heritability for verbal ability, but not non-verbal ability, in early childhood within the context of higher HC.
Berry, 2016 [25]
 
Relationship between increased household disorganisation and decreased cognitive and social outcomes was somewhat moderated by childcare hours (i.e. greater childcare hours attenuated the relationship)
Brown, 2008 [42]
Sleep problems partially mediated the relationship between HC and hopeless/helpless responses to academic challenge.
 
Corapci, 2002 [88]
Overall neither parental mood nor self-efficacy was found to mediate the relationship between HC and parenting.
 
Deater-Deckard, 2012 [12]
 
The relationship between HC and maternal EF was moderated by SES, meaning the adverse effects of HC on maternal EF may be particularly important within socioeconomically distressed contexts.
Fisher, 2018 [80]
 
The quality of classroom climate across 3 years of elementary school was not found to moderate the relationship between HC and adolescent outcomes.
Khatiwada, 2018 [118]
HC was positively associated with weight status at 12 months of age; however, this relationship was not mediated by breastfeeding, sleep, or screen time.
 
Kretschmer, 2009 [91]
The relationship between HC and sibling relationship quality was mediated by parenting, including maternal warmth and paternal harsh discipline.
The relationship between HC and sibling relationship quality was moderated by maternal harsh discipline.
Lemery-Chalfant, 2013 [61]
Lower HC was associated with higher effortful control in children, and this association was genetically mediated.
 
Martin, 2012 [6]
Lack of routines was associated with lower receptive vocab and delayed gratification. In homes with the TV usually on, children had greater aggression scores and attention problems. The association between routines and receptive vocab was partially mediated by provision of learning materials. The association between lack of routines and delayed gratification was not mediated by maternal warmth or provision of learning materials, suggesting routines in and of themselves were associated with development of early self-regulation.
 
Miller, 2017 [63]
 
HC was negatively associated with emotion regulation, but this relationship was not moderated by cortisol levels. The relationship between routines and emotion regulation was moderated by cortisol level, meaning that lack of routines was more strongly associated with poor emotion regulation in children with lower cortisol output.
Mills-Koonce, 2016 [64]
Maternal sensitivity and harsh parenting mediated the relationship between HC and child behaviours.
 
Supplee, 2007 [71]
Maternal report of HC was associated with children’s externalising behaviour at age 4 years, even after controlling for SES and ethnicity.
HC was positively associated with teacher report of externalising problems at school at age 5.5 years. Maternal monitoring was not found to moderate the effects of HC on externalising behaviour.
Tucker, 2015 [74]
 
HC predicted less positive adolescent beliefs about mastery, future obstacles, and having a successful career; mother’s hostility moderated the relationship between HC and future obstacles and stress about the transition to emerging adulthood.
Vernon-Feagans, 2012 [85]
Household disorganisation was associated with variance in receptive and expressive language; parenting partially mediated this relationship.
 
Vernon-Feagans, 2016 [76]
Disorganisation in the home was indirectly associated with behavioural regulation through intermediate impacts on parenting behaviours and children’s early executive function skills.
 
Zvara, 2014 [100]
Cumulative family disorganisation, but not cumulative family instability, was found to have a significant indirect on children’s representation of family dysfunction through parenting behaviours. As such, the proximal effects of daily disorganisation appeared to outweigh the effects of periodic instability overtime.
 

Discussion

The aim of this scoping review was to provide a general summary of the extant research investigating household chaos in children and their families, with respect to (1) measurement tools used, (2) study details, and (3) outcomes assessed. The review found that research to date predominantly relied on either the short- or long-form version of the CHAOS scale, was conducted in either the U.S or U.K, and utilised primarily cross-sectional or longitudinal study designs. Almost a quarter of the research was undertaken in large, nationally representative samples of over 1000 study participants, and mostly focussed on young, healthy children. Both direct and indirect relationships between household chaos and outcomes were investigated, with the majority of studies assessing the relationship between household chaos and socio-emotional and behavioural outcomes. While an in-depth analysis of findings and assessment of bias was beyond the scope of this review, a preliminary summation of the research showed consistency in the evidence for significant relationships between household chaos and adverse effects across seven categories of outcomes in diverse populations with respect to age, disease status, and SES. To the best of our knowledge, this scoping review is the first review to map the literature within this field.
The review demonstrated that household chaos appears to influence a range of child, parent, and family outcomes. The breadth of these findings are not surprising given the diverse ways household chaos potentially interferes with processes in the home environment known to support healthy child development [125]. For example, evidence suggests that parenting behaviours and parent-child interactions are compromised within chaotic home environments [12, 39, 89, 126]. Parents in chaotic homes have been shown to be less responsive, less stimulating, more likely to interfere with children’s attempts at exploration, less likely to provide scaffolding, and more likely to engage in harsher discipline [2]. Not surprisingly, parenting was found to mediate the relationship between household chaos and a number of outcomes, including sibling relationship quality [91], child behaviours [64], receptive and expressive language [85], and children’s representation of family dysfunction [100]. Yet, it was also shown that even in the presence of positive parenting, such as maternal monitoring, the adverse effects of household chaos remain, suggesting that the negative effects of living in a chaotic home environment may not always be overcome by positive parenting practices [71].
It has also been suggested that the mechanism through which chaos impacts on parents is through increasing levels of stress and distraction, resulting in reduced regulatory functioning of the prefrontal lobe, and thus rendering even parents with normal to high emotional regulation and cognitive control compromised in their ability to parent effectively [127]. For example, household chaos has been shown to moderate the relationship between parenting behaviours and child outcomes, whereby chaos exacerbates the effects of negative parenting behaviours and undermines the effects of positive parenting behaviours [4, 24, 26, 58, 73, 92, 97].
Household chaos has also consistently been found to impact on measures of stress physiology in young children, and thus may represent a form of toxic, albeit low-level, stress [106, 128]. Toxic stress in childhood has been linked with increased risk of negative health outcomes in later life [129], and one potential pathway connecting toxic stress in childhood and adult health is through an altered stress response [130]. Exposure to household chaos during the preschool years was shown to be associated with a blunted diurnal cortisol slope in middle childhood [112], and further, household chaos was found to partially mediate the relationship between low SES and cortisol levels [107]. These findings are concerning as a blunted diurnal cortisol slope, which is considered maladaptive, has been identified as a precursor to a number of diseases and disorders in adulthood [131]. As such, household chaos may signify an aspect of toxic stress in childhood that should be considered by public health researchers.
It may also be that household chaos impedes child development directly through effects on attention allocation and information-processing skills [1]. In the context of a home environment with high frequency or high levels of distractions and background noise, the child develops adaptive techniques for filtering out stimulation; however, these techniques may not be selective, meaning that stimulation that facilitates development is also inadvertently filtered out [132]. Importantly, technological advances in the last 10 years may have created greater opportunities for children to be exposed to background media stimulation. For emphasis, it is possible that newer media devices, such as Smartphones, have increased the level of background media distraction that children are exposed to in the modern home environment. These newer devices not only interfere with child attention processes, but can also reduce responsive parenting behaviours. A recent review found that increased mobile connectivity distracts parents from parent-child interactions, and that distracted parents are more likely to be less responsive and sensitive to the needs of their children [133]. Further, a 2018 cross-sectional study in preschool-aged children (n = 385) found that greater levels of household chaos were associated with increased total screen use in pre-schoolers and screen-use behaviours related to disrupted nighttime sleep [134]. Future research will need to determine whether screen use mediates the relationship between household chaos and outcomes, or alternatively, whether new media devices contribute to the household chaos construct itself.
We also identified a number of studies demonstrating the mediating role of household chaos between risk factors and adverse child outcomes. Of particular interest was the potential role household chaos plays in explaining, at least in part, the relationship between SES and chid outcomes. For example, household chaos was found to mediate the relationship between low SES and daily cortisol output [107], socioemotional adjustment [14], academic achievement (via the disorganisation pathway rather than the instability pathway) [26], and emotional problems [70], but not conduct problems in children with ADHD [47]. More generally, household chaos was also shown to mediate relationships between child sleep and anxiety [52], child behavioural problems and sleep resistance [13], and maternal self-regulation and infant distress to limitations [39].
Finally, household chaos was also shown to moderate the relationship between risk/protective factors and outcomes. For example, children in high chaos homes were shown to be more vulnerable to risk factors, such as hostile parenting [82], and less likely to benefit from positive parenting practices, such as parental control [59]. Alternatively, children in lower chaos homes appeared more likely to benefit from protective factors, such as parental monitoring [81]. A systematic review of these studies is required to document the evidence supporting the mediating and moderating roles of household chaos between risk/protective factors and child outcomes.
This review had a number of strengths and limitations. The scoping review methodology allowed us to map a heterogeneous research area, providing an overview of research within a field that has previously escaped comprehensive review [19]. It also enabled us to summarise research using a variety of study designs and methodologies, and assess a large number of outcomes across several categories. As a result, this scoping review provides a comprehensive overview of published evidence investigating the construct of household chaos, with no limitations on study design, outcomes of interest, context, or age groups. Further, the review appears timely, given the potential for an increasing level of chaos in the daily lives of families [115], in addition to the increasing number of studies published in the field in recent years.
However, scoping reviews are not without their limitations. While we provided a general narrative overview of findings, more work is required to further analyse and synthesise the findings reported in the included studies. Additionally, we did not conduct an assessment of study quality, in line with scoping review guidelines [15], and therefore it is not possible to comment on the quality of the research reported herein, and further, while we followed recommended guidelines for conducting scoping reviews, we did not undertake the optional consultation process. The considerable heterogeneity across studies, with respect to study designs, methodologies, and outcomes of interest, may also be considered a weakness, as it did not allow in-depth synthesis of the findings, and therefore difficulty in identifying nuances in the research, such as critical windows of exposure. Finally, our reliance on published data only subjected the review to publication bias; however, the decision was made to exclude the grey literature, given the large number of published studies identified by the original search.
Our findings highlighted that the majority of research in the area has been conducted in U.S and U.K populations, and as such, studies from other countries may be needed to better understand how chaotic home environments affect families within different cultural contexts. Research is also needed to assess whether household chaos has been increasing in recent years, and if so, if this increase is across the population or only within specific sub-groups. Drivers of this potential change should also be investigated. For example, widespread social changes may have reduced a family’s ability to engage in routines, an important pathway by which household chaos may negatively affect children [6]. Families today are more likely to have two parents who work [135], and children who are cared for outside the family home [136]. These activities may increase family disorganisation and environmental confusion, through increasing logistical demands, feelings of hurriedness, and increasing perceptions of time scarcity, which in turn may result in decreased engagement in important family routines and rituals [137]. Finally, while high levels of household chaos negatively influence children and families, it is unknown whether there is an ideal lower limit of household chaos, below which adverse effects of an ‘overly structured household’ may occur.
Identifying, targeting, and effectively reducing household chaos may offer a unique course of action for (1) improving child, parent, and family outcomes, (2) tackling social, behavioural, cognitive, and health problems linked with low SES in childhood [107], and (3) increasing the impact of family-based programmes designed to improve child outcomes. Yet we do not believe it is possible to make recommendations for practice as our scoping review did not assess the methodological quality of the included research [15]. Before recommendations can be made, we suggest the need for systematic reviews that focus on specific outcomes of interest, pathways linking chaos and outcomes, and the mediating and moderating role of household chaos between risk and protective factors and child outcomes. These reviews would allow for an assessment of study quality and would enable recommendations to be made about how findings could inform practice. Studies are also needed to assess whether it is possible to reduce household chaos. To the best of our knowledge, no interventions have been undertaken with the primary aim of targeting household chaos and as such no tools are currently available that have demonstrable effectiveness in reducing chaos in the family home. Further, it remains to be seen whether a reduction in household chaos actually translates into positive outcomes for children, parents, and families.

Conclusions

Our review identified a diverse body of literature investigating the construct of household chaos. We found that chaotic home environments appear to correlate with a broad spectrum of adverse child, parent, and family outcomes, potentially describe, at least to some extent, the relationship between low SES and adverse outcomes, undermine positive parenting behaviours, and exacerbate negative parenting behaviours. Future research is needed to investigate whether household chaos has been increasing in recent years, what factors may have driven the hypothesised increase in household chaos, whether newer mobile media devices create greater opportunity for the experience of household chaos, and, if so, how to capture this in a new or updated measurement tool. The effects of the construct on outcomes also need to be investigated in other cultural contexts, and programmes developed to not only investigate how household chaos can effectively be reduced, but also assess whether a reduction in household chaos translates into improved outcomes. Before recommendations to inform practice can be made, we propose the undertaking of systematic reviews looking at specific outcomes of interest and the pathways through which household chaos impacts on child, parent, and family outcomes.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.
Not applicable.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Ackerman BP, Brown ED. Physical and psychosocial turmoil in the home and cognitive development; 2010.CrossRef Ackerman BP, Brown ED. Physical and psychosocial turmoil in the home and cognitive development; 2010.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Deater-Deckard K, Mullineaux PY, Beekman C, Petrill SA, Schatschneider C, Thompson LA. Conduct problems, IQ, and household chaos: a longitudinal multi-informant study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50(10):1301–8.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Deater-Deckard K, Mullineaux PY, Beekman C, Petrill SA, Schatschneider C, Thompson LA. Conduct problems, IQ, and household chaos: a longitudinal multi-informant study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50(10):1301–8.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Coldwell J, Pike A, Dunn J. Household chaos–links with parenting and child behaviour. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(11):1116–22.PubMedCrossRef Coldwell J, Pike A, Dunn J. Household chaos–links with parenting and child behaviour. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(11):1116–22.PubMedCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Wachs TD, Chan A. Specificity of environmental action, as seen in environmental correlates of infants’ communication performance. Child Dev. 1986;v57:1464–74. Wachs TD, Chan A. Specificity of environmental action, as seen in environmental correlates of infants’ communication performance. Child Dev. 1986;v57:1464–74.
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin A, Razza RA, Brooks-Gunn J. Specifying the links between household chaos and preschool children’s development. Early Child Dev Care. 2012;182(10):1247–63.PubMedCrossRef Martin A, Razza RA, Brooks-Gunn J. Specifying the links between household chaos and preschool children’s development. Early Child Dev Care. 2012;182(10):1247–63.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnson AD, Martin A, Brooks-Gunn J, Petrill SA. Order in the house! Associations among household chaos, the home literacy environment, maternal reading ability, and children’s early reading. Merrill Palmer Q (Wayne State Univ Press). 2008;54(4):445.CrossRef Johnson AD, Martin A, Brooks-Gunn J, Petrill SA. Order in the house! Associations among household chaos, the home literacy environment, maternal reading ability, and children’s early reading. Merrill Palmer Q (Wayne State Univ Press). 2008;54(4):445.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Aiello JR, Nicosia G, Thompson DE. Physiological, social, and behavioral consequences of crowding on children and adolescents. Child Dev. 1979;v50:195–202. Aiello JR, Nicosia G, Thompson DE. Physiological, social, and behavioral consequences of crowding on children and adolescents. Child Dev. 1979;v50:195–202.
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Boles RE, Halbower AC, Daniels S, Gunnarsdottir T, Whitesell N, Johnson SL. Family chaos and child functioning in relation to sleep problems among children at risk for obesity. Behav Sleep Med. 2016;v182:1–15. Boles RE, Halbower AC, Daniels S, Gunnarsdottir T, Whitesell N, Johnson SL. Family chaos and child functioning in relation to sleep problems among children at risk for obesity. Behav Sleep Med. 2016;v182:1–15.
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Dumas JE, Nissley J, Nordstrom A, Smith EP, Prinz RJ, Levine DW. Home chaos: sociodemographic, parenting, interactional, and child correlates. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2005;34(1):93–104.PubMedCrossRef Dumas JE, Nissley J, Nordstrom A, Smith EP, Prinz RJ, Levine DW. Home chaos: sociodemographic, parenting, interactional, and child correlates. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2005;34(1):93–104.PubMedCrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Hart SA, Petrill SA, Deckard KD, Thompson LA. SES and CHAOS as environmental mediators of cognitive ability: a longitudinal genetic analysis. Intelligence. 2007;35(3):233–42.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hart SA, Petrill SA, Deckard KD, Thompson LA. SES and CHAOS as environmental mediators of cognitive ability: a longitudinal genetic analysis. Intelligence. 2007;35(3):233–42.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Deater-Deckard K, Chen N, Wang Z, Bell MA. Socioeconomic risk moderates the link between household chaos and maternal executive function. J Fam Psychol. 2012;26(3):391–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Deater-Deckard K, Chen N, Wang Z, Bell MA. Socioeconomic risk moderates the link between household chaos and maternal executive function. J Fam Psychol. 2012;26(3):391–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Boles RE, Halbower AC, Daniels S, Gunnarsdottir T, Whitesell N, Johnson SL. Family chaos and child functioning in relation to sleep problems among children at risk for obesity. Behav Sleep Med. 2017;15(2):114–28.PubMedCrossRef Boles RE, Halbower AC, Daniels S, Gunnarsdottir T, Whitesell N, Johnson SL. Family chaos and child functioning in relation to sleep problems among children at risk for obesity. Behav Sleep Med. 2017;15(2):114–28.PubMedCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Evans GW, Gonnella C, Marcynyszyn LA, Gentile L, Salpekar N. The role of chaos in poverty and children’s socioemotional adjustment. Psychol Sci. 2005;16(7):560–5.PubMedCrossRef Evans GW, Gonnella C, Marcynyszyn LA, Gentile L, Salpekar N. The role of chaos in poverty and children’s socioemotional adjustment. Psychol Sci. 2005;16(7):560–5.PubMedCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Joanna Briggs Institute. Joanna Briggs Institute Reviewers’ Manual: 2017 edition. Australia: The Joanna Briggs Institute; 2017. Joanna Briggs Institute. Joanna Briggs Institute Reviewers’ Manual: 2017 edition. Australia: The Joanna Briggs Institute; 2017.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Arksey H, O'Malley L. Scoping studies: towards a methodological framework. Int J Soc Res Methodol. 2005;8(1):19–32.CrossRef Arksey H, O'Malley L. Scoping studies: towards a methodological framework. Int J Soc Res Methodol. 2005;8(1):19–32.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Colquhoun HL, Levac D, O'Brien KK, Straus S, Tricco AC, Perrier L, et al. Scoping reviews: time for clarity in definition, methods, and reporting. J Clin Epidemiol. 2014;67(12):1291–4.PubMedCrossRef Colquhoun HL, Levac D, O'Brien KK, Straus S, Tricco AC, Perrier L, et al. Scoping reviews: time for clarity in definition, methods, and reporting. J Clin Epidemiol. 2014;67(12):1291–4.PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Peters MD, Godfrey CM, Khalil H, McInerney P, Parker D, Soares CB. Guidance for conducting systematic scoping reviews. Int J Evid Based Healthc. 2015;13(3):141–6.PubMedCrossRef Peters MD, Godfrey CM, Khalil H, McInerney P, Parker D, Soares CB. Guidance for conducting systematic scoping reviews. Int J Evid Based Healthc. 2015;13(3):141–6.PubMedCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Tricco AC, Lillie E, Zarin W, O'Brien KK, Colquhoun H, Levac D, et al. PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR): checklist and explanation. Ann Intern Med. 2018;169(7):467–73.PubMedCrossRef Tricco AC, Lillie E, Zarin W, O'Brien KK, Colquhoun H, Levac D, et al. PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR): checklist and explanation. Ann Intern Med. 2018;169(7):467–73.PubMedCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Larsen K. Organized chaos: daily routines as a potential mechanism linking household chaos and child behavior problems; 2019. Larsen K. Organized chaos: daily routines as a potential mechanism linking household chaos and child behavior problems; 2019.
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Matheny AP, Wachs TD, Ludwig JL, Phillips K. Bringing order out of chaos: psychometric characteristics of the confusion, hubbub, and order scale. J Appl Dev Psychol. 1995;16(3):429–44.CrossRef Matheny AP, Wachs TD, Ludwig JL, Phillips K. Bringing order out of chaos: psychometric characteristics of the confusion, hubbub, and order scale. J Appl Dev Psychol. 1995;16(3):429–44.CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Petrill SA, Pike A, Price T, Plomin R. Chaos in the home and socioeconomic status are associated with cognitive development in early childhood: environmental mediators identified in a genetic design. Intelligence. 2004;32(5):445–60.CrossRef Petrill SA, Pike A, Price T, Plomin R. Chaos in the home and socioeconomic status are associated with cognitive development in early childhood: environmental mediators identified in a genetic design. Intelligence. 2004;32(5):445–60.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Asbury K, Dunn JF, Plomin R. Birthweight-discordance and differences in early parenting relate to monozygotic twin differences in behaviour problems and academic achievement at age 7. Dev Sci. 2006;9(2):F22–31.PubMedCrossRef Asbury K, Dunn JF, Plomin R. Birthweight-discordance and differences in early parenting relate to monozygotic twin differences in behaviour problems and academic achievement at age 7. Dev Sci. 2006;9(2):F22–31.PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Berry D, Blair C, Willoughby M, Garrett-Peters P, Vernon-Feagans L, Mills-Koonce WR. Household chaos and children’s cognitive and socio-emotional development in early childhood: does childcare play a buffering role? Early Child Res Q. 2016;34:115–27.PubMedCrossRef Berry D, Blair C, Willoughby M, Garrett-Peters P, Vernon-Feagans L, Mills-Koonce WR. Household chaos and children’s cognitive and socio-emotional development in early childhood: does childcare play a buffering role? Early Child Res Q. 2016;34:115–27.PubMedCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Garrett-Peters PT, Mokrova I, Vernon-Feagans L, Willoughby M, Pan Y. The role of household chaos in understanding relations between early poverty and children’s academic achievement. Early Child Res Q. 2016;37:16–25.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Garrett-Peters PT, Mokrova I, Vernon-Feagans L, Willoughby M, Pan Y. The role of household chaos in understanding relations between early poverty and children’s academic achievement. Early Child Res Q. 2016;37:16–25.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Hanscombe KB, Haworth CM, Davis OS, Jaffee SR, Plomin R. Chaotic homes and school achievement: a twin study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52(11):1212–20.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hanscombe KB, Haworth CM, Davis OS, Jaffee SR, Plomin R. Chaotic homes and school achievement: a twin study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52(11):1212–20.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Hur E, Buettner CK, Jeon L. Parental depressive symptoms and children’s school-readiness: the indirect effect of household chaos. J Child Fam Stud. 2015;24(11):3462–73.CrossRef Hur E, Buettner CK, Jeon L. Parental depressive symptoms and children’s school-readiness: the indirect effect of household chaos. J Child Fam Stud. 2015;24(11):3462–73.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Pike A, Iervolino AC, Eley TC, Price TS, Plomin R. Environmental risk and young children’s cognitive and behavioral development. Int J Behav Dev. 2006;30(1):55–66.CrossRef Pike A, Iervolino AC, Eley TC, Price TS, Plomin R. Environmental risk and young children’s cognitive and behavioral development. Int J Behav Dev. 2006;30(1):55–66.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Shamama-tus-Sabah S, Gilani N. Household chaos and its association with maternal education, family system, and children’s academic achievement in Pakistani culture. Pak J Psychol Res. 2010;25(1):19–30. Shamama-tus-Sabah S, Gilani N. Household chaos and its association with maternal education, family system, and children’s academic achievement in Pakistani culture. Pak J Psychol Res. 2010;25(1):19–30.
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Shamama-tus-Sabah S, Gilani N. Household chaos, attention and school problems in primary school children. J Behav Sci. 2011;21(1):68. Shamama-tus-Sabah S, Gilani N. Household chaos, attention and school problems in primary school children. J Behav Sci. 2011;21(1):68.
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Shamama-tus-Sabah S, Gilani N, Wachs TD. Relation of home chaos to cognitive performance and behavioral adjustment of Pakistani primary school children. Int J Behav Dev. 2011;35(6):507–16.CrossRef Shamama-tus-Sabah S, Gilani N, Wachs TD. Relation of home chaos to cognitive performance and behavioral adjustment of Pakistani primary school children. Int J Behav Dev. 2011;35(6):507–16.CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Shamama-tus-Sabah S, Gillani N. Conduct problems, social skills, study skills, and home chaos in school children: a correlational study. Pak J Psychol Res. 2011;26(2):201. Shamama-tus-Sabah S, Gillani N. Conduct problems, social skills, study skills, and home chaos in school children: a correlational study. Pak J Psychol Res. 2011;26(2):201.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor J, Ennis CR, Hart SA, Mikolajewski AJ, Schatschneider C. Home environmental and behavioral risk indices for reading achievement. Learn Individ Differ. 2017;57:9–21.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Taylor J, Ennis CR, Hart SA, Mikolajewski AJ, Schatschneider C. Home environmental and behavioral risk indices for reading achievement. Learn Individ Differ. 2017;57:9–21.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor J, Hart SA. A chaotic home environment accounts for the association between respect for rules disposition and reading comprehension: a twin study. Learn Individ Differ. 2014;35:70–7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Taylor J, Hart SA. A chaotic home environment accounts for the association between respect for rules disposition and reading comprehension: a twin study. Learn Individ Differ. 2014;35:70–7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Yarboi J, Compas BE, Brody GH, White D, Rees Patterson J, Ziara K, et al. Association of social-environmental factors with cognitive function in children with sickle cell disease. Child Neuropsychol. 2017;23(3):343–60.PubMedCrossRef Yarboi J, Compas BE, Brody GH, White D, Rees Patterson J, Ziara K, et al. Association of social-environmental factors with cognitive function in children with sickle cell disease. Child Neuropsychol. 2017;23(3):343–60.PubMedCrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Asbury K, Dunn JF, Pike A, Plomin R. Nonshared environmental influences on individual differences in early behavioral development: a monozygotic twin differences study. Child Dev. 2003;74(3):933–43.PubMedCrossRef Asbury K, Dunn JF, Pike A, Plomin R. Nonshared environmental influences on individual differences in early behavioral development: a monozygotic twin differences study. Child Dev. 2003;74(3):933–43.PubMedCrossRef
38.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Bridgett DJ, Burt NM, Laake LM, Oddi KB. Maternal self-regulation, relationship adjustment, and home chaos: contributions to infant negative emotionality. Infant Behav Dev. 2013;36(4):534–47.PubMedCrossRef Bridgett DJ, Burt NM, Laake LM, Oddi KB. Maternal self-regulation, relationship adjustment, and home chaos: contributions to infant negative emotionality. Infant Behav Dev. 2013;36(4):534–47.PubMedCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Brieant A, Holmes CJ, Deater-Deckard K, King-Casas B, Kim-Spoon J. Household chaos as a context for intergenerational transmission of executive functioning. J Adolesc. 2017;58:40–8.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Brieant A, Holmes CJ, Deater-Deckard K, King-Casas B, Kim-Spoon J. Household chaos as a context for intergenerational transmission of executive functioning. J Adolesc. 2017;58:40–8.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Brown DD, Weatherholt TN, Burns BM. Understanding parent reports of children’s attention behaviors: role of children’s attention skills, temperament, and home environment. J Early Child Infant Psychol. 2010;6:41–58. Brown DD, Weatherholt TN, Burns BM. Understanding parent reports of children’s attention behaviors: role of children’s attention skills, temperament, and home environment. J Early Child Infant Psychol. 2010;6:41–58.
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Brown ED, Low CM. Chaotic living conditions and sleep problems associated with children’s responses to academic challenge. J Fam Psychol. 2008;22(6):920.PubMedCrossRef Brown ED, Low CM. Chaotic living conditions and sleep problems associated with children’s responses to academic challenge. J Fam Psychol. 2008;22(6):920.PubMedCrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Calam R, Jones S, Sanders MR, Dempsey R, Sadhnani V. Parenting and the emotional and behavioural adjustment of young children in families with a parent with bipolar disorder. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2012;40(4):425–37.PubMedCrossRef Calam R, Jones S, Sanders MR, Dempsey R, Sadhnani V. Parenting and the emotional and behavioural adjustment of young children in families with a parent with bipolar disorder. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2012;40(4):425–37.PubMedCrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Chatterjee A, Gillman MW, Wong MD. Chaos, hubbub, and order scale and health risk behaviors in adolescents in Los Angeles. J Pediatr. 2015;167(6):1415–21.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Chatterjee A, Gillman MW, Wong MD. Chaos, hubbub, and order scale and health risk behaviors in adolescents in Los Angeles. J Pediatr. 2015;167(6):1415–21.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Chen N, Deater-Deckard K, Bell MA. The role of temperament by family environment interactions in child maladjustment. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2014;42(8):1251–62.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Chen N, Deater-Deckard K, Bell MA. The role of temperament by family environment interactions in child maladjustment. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2014;42(8):1251–62.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Farbiash T, Berger A, Atzaba-Poria N, Auerbach J. Prediction of preschool aggression from DRD4 risk, parental ADHD symptoms, and home chaos. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2014;42(3):489–99.PubMedCrossRef Farbiash T, Berger A, Atzaba-Poria N, Auerbach J. Prediction of preschool aggression from DRD4 risk, parental ADHD symptoms, and home chaos. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2014;42(3):489–99.PubMedCrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Flouri E, Midouhas E, Ruddy A, Moulton V. The role of socio-economic disadvantage in the development of comorbid emotional and conduct problems in children with ADHD. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;v26:1–10. Flouri E, Midouhas E, Ruddy A, Moulton V. The role of socio-economic disadvantage in the development of comorbid emotional and conduct problems in children with ADHD. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;v26:1–10.
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Flouri E, Tsivrikos D, Akhtar R, Midouhas E. Neighbourhood, school and family determinants of children’s aspirations in primary school. J Vocat Behav. 2015;87:71–9.CrossRef Flouri E, Tsivrikos D, Akhtar R, Midouhas E. Neighbourhood, school and family determinants of children’s aspirations in primary school. J Vocat Behav. 2015;87:71–9.CrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Fontaine NMG, McCrory EJP, Boivin M, Moffitt TE, Viding E. Predictors and outcomes of joint trajectories of callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. J Abnorm Psychol. 2011;120(3):730–42.PubMedCrossRef Fontaine NMG, McCrory EJP, Boivin M, Moffitt TE, Viding E. Predictors and outcomes of joint trajectories of callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. J Abnorm Psychol. 2011;120(3):730–42.PubMedCrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Fuller-Rowell TE, Evans GW, Paul E, Curtis DS. The role of poverty and chaos in the development of task persistence among adolescents. J Res Adolesc. 2015;25(4):606–13.CrossRef Fuller-Rowell TE, Evans GW, Paul E, Curtis DS. The role of poverty and chaos in the development of task persistence among adolescents. J Res Adolesc. 2015;25(4):606–13.CrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Gould KL, Coventry WL, Olson RK, Byrne B. Gene-environment interactions in ADHD: the roles of SES and chaos. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2017;v46:1–13. Gould KL, Coventry WL, Olson RK, Byrne B. Gene-environment interactions in ADHD: the roles of SES and chaos. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2017;v46:1–13.
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Gregory AM, Eley TC, O'Connor TG, Rijsdijk FV, Plomin R. Family influences on the association between sleep problems and anxiety in a large sample of pre-school aged twins. Pers Individ Differ. 2005;39(8):1337–48.CrossRef Gregory AM, Eley TC, O'Connor TG, Rijsdijk FV, Plomin R. Family influences on the association between sleep problems and anxiety in a large sample of pre-school aged twins. Pers Individ Differ. 2005;39(8):1337–48.CrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Gunzenhauser C, Saalbach H, von Suchodoletz A. Boys have not caught up, family influences still continue: influences on executive functioning and behavioral self-regulation in elementary students in Germany. PsyCh J. 2017;6(3):205–18.PubMedCrossRef Gunzenhauser C, Saalbach H, von Suchodoletz A. Boys have not caught up, family influences still continue: influences on executive functioning and behavioral self-regulation in elementary students in Germany. PsyCh J. 2017;6(3):205–18.PubMedCrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Hannigan LJ, McAdams TA, Eley TC. Developmental change in the association between adolescent depressive symptoms and the home environment: results from a longitudinal, genetically informative investigation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58(7):787–97. Hannigan LJ, McAdams TA, Eley TC. Developmental change in the association between adolescent depressive symptoms and the home environment: results from a longitudinal, genetically informative investigation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58(7):787–97.
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Hardaway CR, Wilson MN, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ. Family functioning and externalizing behaviour among low-income children: self-regulation as a mediator. Infant Child Dev. 2012;21(1):67–84.PubMedCrossRef Hardaway CR, Wilson MN, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ. Family functioning and externalizing behaviour among low-income children: self-regulation as a mediator. Infant Child Dev. 2012;21(1):67–84.PubMedCrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Human LJ, Dirks MA, DeLongis A, Chen E. Congruence and incongruence in adolescents’ and parents’ perceptions of the family: using response surface analysis to examine links with adolescents’ psychological adjustment. J Youth Adolesc. 2016;45(10):2022–35.PubMedCrossRef Human LJ, Dirks MA, DeLongis A, Chen E. Congruence and incongruence in adolescents’ and parents’ perceptions of the family: using response surface analysis to examine links with adolescents’ psychological adjustment. J Youth Adolesc. 2016;45(10):2022–35.PubMedCrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Jaffee SR, Hanscombe KB, Haworth CM, Davis OS, Plomin R. Chaotic homes and children’s disruptive behavior: a longitudinal cross-lagged twin study. Psychol Sci. 2012;23(6):643–50.PubMedCrossRef Jaffee SR, Hanscombe KB, Haworth CM, Davis OS, Plomin R. Chaotic homes and children’s disruptive behavior: a longitudinal cross-lagged twin study. Psychol Sci. 2012;23(6):643–50.PubMedCrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Kahn RE, Deater-Deckard K, King-Casas B, Kim-Spoon J. Intergenerational similarity in callous-unemotional traits: contributions of hostile parenting and household chaos during adolescence. Psychiatry Res. 2016;246:815–20.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kahn RE, Deater-Deckard K, King-Casas B, Kim-Spoon J. Intergenerational similarity in callous-unemotional traits: contributions of hostile parenting and household chaos during adolescence. Psychiatry Res. 2016;246:815–20.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim-Spoon J, Maciejewski D, Lee J, Deater-Deckard K, King-Casas B. Longitudinal associations among family environment, neural cognitive control, and social competence among adolescents. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2017;26:69–76.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kim-Spoon J, Maciejewski D, Lee J, Deater-Deckard K, King-Casas B. Longitudinal associations among family environment, neural cognitive control, and social competence among adolescents. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2017;26:69–76.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Laurent HK, Neiderhiser JM, Natsuaki MN, Shaw DS, Fisher PA, Reiss D, et al. Stress system development from age 4.5 to 6: family environment predictors and adjustment implications of HPA activity stability versus change. Dev Psychobiol. 2014;56(3):340–54.PubMedCrossRef Laurent HK, Neiderhiser JM, Natsuaki MN, Shaw DS, Fisher PA, Reiss D, et al. Stress system development from age 4.5 to 6: family environment predictors and adjustment implications of HPA activity stability versus change. Dev Psychobiol. 2014;56(3):340–54.PubMedCrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Lemery-Chalfant K, Kao K, Swann G, Goldsmith H. Childhood temperament: passive gene-environment correlation, gene-environment interaction, and the hidden importance of the family environment. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25(1):51–63.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Lemery-Chalfant K, Kao K, Swann G, Goldsmith H. Childhood temperament: passive gene-environment correlation, gene-environment interaction, and the hidden importance of the family environment. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25(1):51–63.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Midouhas E, Yogaratnam A, Flouri E, Charman T. Psychopathology trajectories of children with autism spectrum disorder: the role of family poverty and parenting. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52(10):1057–65.e1.PubMedCrossRef Midouhas E, Yogaratnam A, Flouri E, Charman T. Psychopathology trajectories of children with autism spectrum disorder: the role of family poverty and parenting. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52(10):1057–65.e1.PubMedCrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Miller AL, Song J-H, Sturza J, Lumeng JC, Rosenblum K, Kaciroti N, et al. Child cortisol moderates the association between family routines and emotion regulation in low-income children. Dev Psychobiol. 2017;59(1):99–110.PubMedCrossRef Miller AL, Song J-H, Sturza J, Lumeng JC, Rosenblum K, Kaciroti N, et al. Child cortisol moderates the association between family routines and emotion regulation in low-income children. Dev Psychobiol. 2017;59(1):99–110.PubMedCrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Mills-Koonce W, Willoughby MT, Garrett-Peters P, Wagner N, Vernon-Feagans L. The interplay among socioeconomic status, household chaos, and parenting in the prediction of child conduct problems and callous-unemotional behaviors. Dev Psychopathol. 2016;28(3):757–71.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral Mills-Koonce W, Willoughby MT, Garrett-Peters P, Wagner N, Vernon-Feagans L. The interplay among socioeconomic status, household chaos, and parenting in the prediction of child conduct problems and callous-unemotional behaviors. Dev Psychopathol. 2016;28(3):757–71.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Oliver BR, Pike A, Plomin R. Nonshared environmental influences on teacher-reported behaviour problems: monozygotic twin differences in perceptions of the classroom. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(6):646–53.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Oliver BR, Pike A, Plomin R. Nonshared environmental influences on teacher-reported behaviour problems: monozygotic twin differences in perceptions of the classroom. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(6):646–53.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Panico L, Becares L, Webb EA. Exploring household dynamics: the reciprocal effects of parent and child characteristics. Longitudinal Life Course Stud. 2014;5(1):42–55.CrossRef Panico L, Becares L, Webb EA. Exploring household dynamics: the reciprocal effects of parent and child characteristics. Longitudinal Life Course Stud. 2014;5(1):42–55.CrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Raver C, Blair C, Garrett-Peters P. Poverty, household chaos, and interparental aggression predict children’s ability to recognize and modulate negative emotions. Dev Psychopathol. 2015;27(3):695–708.PubMedCrossRef Raver C, Blair C, Garrett-Peters P. Poverty, household chaos, and interparental aggression predict children’s ability to recognize and modulate negative emotions. Dev Psychopathol. 2015;27(3):695–708.PubMedCrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Shamama-tus-Sabah S, Gilani N, Kamal A, Batool S. Chaotic home conditions and children’s adjustment: study of gender differences. Pak J Psychol Res. 2012;27(2):297–313. Shamama-tus-Sabah S, Gilani N, Kamal A, Batool S. Chaotic home conditions and children’s adjustment: study of gender differences. Pak J Psychol Res. 2012;27(2):297–313.
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Shapero BG, Steinberg L. Emotional reactivity and exposure to household stress in childhood predict psychological problems in adolescence. J Youth Adolesc. 2013;42(10):1573–82.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Shapero BG, Steinberg L. Emotional reactivity and exposure to household stress in childhood predict psychological problems in adolescence. J Youth Adolesc. 2013;42(10):1573–82.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Shelleby EC, Votruba-Drzal E, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, Wilson MN, Gardner F. Income and children’s behavioral functioning: a sequential mediation analysis. J Fam Psychol. 2014;28(6):936–46.PubMedCrossRef Shelleby EC, Votruba-Drzal E, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, Wilson MN, Gardner F. Income and children’s behavioral functioning: a sequential mediation analysis. J Fam Psychol. 2014;28(6):936–46.PubMedCrossRef
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Supplee LH, Unikel EB, Shaw DS. Physical environmental adversity and the protective role of maternal monitoring in relation to early child conduct problems. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2007;28(2):166–83.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Supplee LH, Unikel EB, Shaw DS. Physical environmental adversity and the protective role of maternal monitoring in relation to early child conduct problems. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2007;28(2):166–83.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Tomalski P, Marczuk K, Pisula E, Malinowska A, Kawa R, Niedźwiecka A. Chaotic home environment is associated with reduced infant processing speed under high task demands. Infant Behav Dev. 2017;48:Part B (p124-133). Tomalski P, Marczuk K, Pisula E, Malinowska A, Kawa R, Niedźwiecka A. Chaotic home environment is associated with reduced infant processing speed under high task demands. Infant Behav Dev. 2017;48:Part B (p124-133).
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Towe-Goodman NR, Stifter CA, Coccia MA, Cox MJ. Interparental aggression, attention skills, and early childhood behavior problems. Dev Psychopathol. 2011;23(02):563–76.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Towe-Goodman NR, Stifter CA, Coccia MA, Cox MJ. Interparental aggression, attention skills, and early childhood behavior problems. Dev Psychopathol. 2011;23(02):563–76.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Tucker CJ, Sharp EH, Van Gundy KT, Rebellon CJ. Household chaos, relationships with parents and adolescents’ future beliefs. J Fam Stud. 2015;v23:1–14. Tucker CJ, Sharp EH, Van Gundy KT, Rebellon CJ. Household chaos, relationships with parents and adolescents’ future beliefs. J Fam Stud. 2015;v23:1–14.
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Valiente C, Lemery-Chalfant K, Reiser M. Pathways to problem behaviors: chaotic homes, parent and child effortful control, and parenting. Soc Dev. 2007;16(2):249–67.CrossRef Valiente C, Lemery-Chalfant K, Reiser M. Pathways to problem behaviors: chaotic homes, parent and child effortful control, and parenting. Soc Dev. 2007;16(2):249–67.CrossRef
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Vernon-Feagans L, Garrett-Peters P, Willoughby M. Predictors of behavioral regulation in kindergarten: household chaos, parenting, and early executive functions. Dev Psychol. 2016;52(3):430–41.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Vernon-Feagans L, Garrett-Peters P, Willoughby M. Predictors of behavioral regulation in kindergarten: household chaos, parenting, and early executive functions. Dev Psychol. 2016;52(3):430–41.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Vilsaint CL, Aiyer SM, Wilson MN, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ. The ecology of early childhood risk: a canonical correlation analysis of children’s adjustment, family, and community context in a high-risk sample. J Prim Prev. 2013;34(4):261–77.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Vilsaint CL, Aiyer SM, Wilson MN, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ. The ecology of early childhood risk: a canonical correlation analysis of children’s adjustment, family, and community context in a high-risk sample. J Prim Prev. 2013;34(4):261–77.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang Z, Deater-Deckard K, Petrill SA, Thompson LA. Externalizing problems, attention regulation, and household chaos: a longitudinal behavioral genetic study. Dev Psychopathol. 2012;24(3):755–69.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wang Z, Deater-Deckard K, Petrill SA, Thompson LA. Externalizing problems, attention regulation, and household chaos: a longitudinal behavioral genetic study. Dev Psychopathol. 2012;24(3):755–69.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Wilkinson PO, Trzaskowski M, Haworth CM, Eley TC. The role of gene-environment correlations and interactions in middle childhood depressive symptoms. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25(1):93–104.PubMedCrossRef Wilkinson PO, Trzaskowski M, Haworth CM, Eley TC. The role of gene-environment correlations and interactions in middle childhood depressive symptoms. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25(1):93–104.PubMedCrossRef
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Fisher JH, Brown JL. A prospective, longitudinal examination of the influence of childhood home and school contexts on psychopathic characteristics in adolescence. J Youth Adolesc. 2018;v47:1–19. Fisher JH, Brown JL. A prospective, longitudinal examination of the influence of childhood home and school contexts on psychopathic characteristics in adolescence. J Youth Adolesc. 2018;v47:1–19.
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Lauharatanahirun N, Maciejewski D, Holmes C, Deater-Deckard K, Kim-Spoon J, King-Casas B. Neural correlates of risk processing among adolescents: influences of parental monitoring and household chaos. Child Dev. 2018;89(3):784–96.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Lauharatanahirun N, Maciejewski D, Holmes C, Deater-Deckard K, Kim-Spoon J, King-Casas B. Neural correlates of risk processing among adolescents: influences of parental monitoring and household chaos. Child Dev. 2018;89(3):784–96.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Tucker CJ, Sharp EH, Van Gundy KT, Rebellon C. Household chaos, hostile parenting, and adolescents’ well-being two years later. J Child Fam Stud. 2018;v27:1–8. Tucker CJ, Sharp EH, Van Gundy KT, Rebellon C. Household chaos, hostile parenting, and adolescents’ well-being two years later. J Child Fam Stud. 2018;v27:1–8.
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Vrijhof CI, van der Voort A, van IJzendoorn MH, Euser S. Stressful family environments and children’s behavioral control: a multimethod test and replication study with twins. J Fam Psychol. 2018;32(1):49.PubMedCrossRef Vrijhof CI, van der Voort A, van IJzendoorn MH, Euser S. Stressful family environments and children’s behavioral control: a multimethod test and replication study with twins. J Fam Psychol. 2018;32(1):49.PubMedCrossRef
84.
Zurück zum Zitat Asbury K, Wachs TD, Plomin R. Environmental moderators of genetic influence on verbal and nonverbal abilities in early childhood. Intelligence. 2005;33(6):643–61.CrossRef Asbury K, Wachs TD, Plomin R. Environmental moderators of genetic influence on verbal and nonverbal abilities in early childhood. Intelligence. 2005;33(6):643–61.CrossRef
85.
Zurück zum Zitat Vernon-Feagans L, Garrett-Peters P, Willoughby M, Mills-Koonce R. Chaos, poverty, and parenting: predictors of early language development. Early Child Res Q. 2012;27(3):339–51.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Vernon-Feagans L, Garrett-Peters P, Willoughby M, Mills-Koonce R. Chaos, poverty, and parenting: predictors of early language development. Early Child Res Q. 2012;27(3):339–51.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
86.
Zurück zum Zitat Atzaba-Poria N, Pike A. Correlates of parental differential treatment: parental and contextual factors during middle childhood. Child Dev. 2008;79(1):217–32.PubMedCrossRef Atzaba-Poria N, Pike A. Correlates of parental differential treatment: parental and contextual factors during middle childhood. Child Dev. 2008;79(1):217–32.PubMedCrossRef
87.
Zurück zum Zitat Barnes J, Gardiner J, Sutcliffe A, Melhuish E. The parenting of preschool children by older mothers in the United Kingdom. Eur J Dev Psychol. 2014;11(4):397–419.CrossRef Barnes J, Gardiner J, Sutcliffe A, Melhuish E. The parenting of preschool children by older mothers in the United Kingdom. Eur J Dev Psychol. 2014;11(4):397–419.CrossRef
88.
Zurück zum Zitat Corapci F, Wachs TD. Does parental mood or efficacy mediate the influence of environmental chaos upon parenting behavior? Merrill-Palmer Q. 2002;48(2):182–201.CrossRef Corapci F, Wachs TD. Does parental mood or efficacy mediate the influence of environmental chaos upon parenting behavior? Merrill-Palmer Q. 2002;48(2):182–201.CrossRef
89.
Zurück zum Zitat Deater-Deckard K, Wang Z, Chen N, Bell MA. Maternal executive function, harsh parenting, and child conduct problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53(10):1084–91.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Deater-Deckard K, Wang Z, Chen N, Bell MA. Maternal executive function, harsh parenting, and child conduct problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53(10):1084–91.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
90.
Zurück zum Zitat Fiese BH, Gundersen C, Koester B, Jones B. Family chaos and lack of mealtime planning is associated with food insecurity in low income households. Econ Hum Biol. 2016;21:147–55.PubMedCrossRef Fiese BH, Gundersen C, Koester B, Jones B. Family chaos and lack of mealtime planning is associated with food insecurity in low income households. Econ Hum Biol. 2016;21:147–55.PubMedCrossRef
91.
Zurück zum Zitat Kretschmer T, Pike A. Young children’s sibling relationship quality: distal and proximal correlates. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50(5):581–9.PubMedCrossRef Kretschmer T, Pike A. Young children’s sibling relationship quality: distal and proximal correlates. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50(5):581–9.PubMedCrossRef
92.
Zurück zum Zitat Mokrova I, O'Brien M, Calkins S, Keane S. Parental ADHD symptomology and ineffective parenting: the connecting link of home chaos. Parent Sci Pract. 2010;10(2):119–35.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mokrova I, O'Brien M, Calkins S, Keane S. Parental ADHD symptomology and ineffective parenting: the connecting link of home chaos. Parent Sci Pract. 2010;10(2):119–35.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
93.
Zurück zum Zitat Nelson JA, O'Brien M, Blankson AN, Calkins SD, Keane SP. Family stress and parental responses to children’s negative emotions: tests of the spillover, crossover, and compensatory hypotheses. J Fam Psychol. 2009;23(5):671–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Nelson JA, O'Brien M, Blankson AN, Calkins SD, Keane SP. Family stress and parental responses to children’s negative emotions: tests of the spillover, crossover, and compensatory hypotheses. J Fam Psychol. 2009;23(5):671–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
94.
Zurück zum Zitat Pike A, Atzaba-Poria N, Kretschmer T. Predictors of parenting: family-wide and child-specific factors. Parent Sci Pract. 2016;16(3):147–63.CrossRef Pike A, Atzaba-Poria N, Kretschmer T. Predictors of parenting: family-wide and child-specific factors. Parent Sci Pract. 2016;16(3):147–63.CrossRef
95.
Zurück zum Zitat Pinard CA, Calloway EE, Fricke HE, Yaroch AL. A cross-sectional exploration of food security, depression, and chaos in low-income households with children. J Appl Res Child Informing Policy Child Risk. 2015;6(2):6. Pinard CA, Calloway EE, Fricke HE, Yaroch AL. A cross-sectional exploration of food security, depression, and chaos in low-income households with children. J Appl Res Child Informing Policy Child Risk. 2015;6(2):6.
96.
Zurück zum Zitat Spilsbury JC, Patel SR, Morris N, Ehayaei A, Intille SS. Household chaos and sleep-disturbing behavior of family members: results of a pilot study of African American early adolescents. Sleep Health. 2017;3(2):84–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Spilsbury JC, Patel SR, Morris N, Ehayaei A, Intille SS. Household chaos and sleep-disturbing behavior of family members: results of a pilot study of African American early adolescents. Sleep Health. 2017;3(2):84–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
97.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang Z, Deater-Deckard K, Bell MA. Household chaos moderates the link between maternal attribution bias and parenting. Parent Sci Pract. 2013;13(4):233–52.CrossRef Wang Z, Deater-Deckard K, Bell MA. Household chaos moderates the link between maternal attribution bias and parenting. Parent Sci Pract. 2013;13(4):233–52.CrossRef
98.
Zurück zum Zitat Whitesell CJ, Teti DM, Crosby B, Kim B-R. Household chaos, sociodemographic risk, coparenting, and parent-infant relations during infants’ first year. J Fam Psychol. 2015;29(2):211.PubMedCrossRef Whitesell CJ, Teti DM, Crosby B, Kim B-R. Household chaos, sociodemographic risk, coparenting, and parent-infant relations during infants’ first year. J Fam Psychol. 2015;29(2):211.PubMedCrossRef
99.
Zurück zum Zitat Wirth A, Reinelt T, Gawrilow C, Schwenck C, Freitag CM, Rauch WA. Examining the relationship between children’s ADHD symptomatology and inadequate parenting: the role of household chaos. J Atten Disord. 2017;v23(5):1087054717692881. Wirth A, Reinelt T, Gawrilow C, Schwenck C, Freitag CM, Rauch WA. Examining the relationship between children’s ADHD symptomatology and inadequate parenting: the role of household chaos. J Atten Disord. 2017;v23(5):1087054717692881.
100.
Zurück zum Zitat Zvara BJ, Mills-Koonce WR, Garrett-Peters P, Wagner NJ, Vernon-Feagans L, Cox M, et al. The mediating role of parenting in the associations between household chaos and children’s representations of family dysfunction. Attach Hum Dev. 2014;16(6):633–55.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Zvara BJ, Mills-Koonce WR, Garrett-Peters P, Wagner NJ, Vernon-Feagans L, Cox M, et al. The mediating role of parenting in the associations between household chaos and children’s representations of family dysfunction. Attach Hum Dev. 2014;16(6):633–55.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
101.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin-Biggers J, Quick V, Zhang M, Jin Y, Byrd-Bredbenner C. Relationships of family conflict, cohesion, and chaos in the home environment on maternal and child food-related behaviours. Matern Child Nutr. 2018;14(2):e12540.PubMedCrossRef Martin-Biggers J, Quick V, Zhang M, Jin Y, Byrd-Bredbenner C. Relationships of family conflict, cohesion, and chaos in the home environment on maternal and child food-related behaviours. Matern Child Nutr. 2018;14(2):e12540.PubMedCrossRef
102.
Zurück zum Zitat MacRae LM, Darlington G, Haines J, Ma DW. Examination of associations between chaos in the home environment, serum cortisol level, and dietary fat intake among parents of preschool-age children. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2017;v42(999):1–4. MacRae LM, Darlington G, Haines J, Ma DW. Examination of associations between chaos in the home environment, serum cortisol level, and dietary fat intake among parents of preschool-age children. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2017;v42(999):1–4.
103.
Zurück zum Zitat Madigan S, Wade M, Plamondon A, Jenkins JM. Trajectories of maternal depressive symptoms in the early childhood period and family-wide clustering of risk. J Affect Disord. 2017;215:49–55.PubMedCrossRef Madigan S, Wade M, Plamondon A, Jenkins JM. Trajectories of maternal depressive symptoms in the early childhood period and family-wide clustering of risk. J Affect Disord. 2017;215:49–55.PubMedCrossRef
105.
106.
Zurück zum Zitat Blair C, Berry D, Mills-Koonce R, Granger D, Investigators FLP. Cumulative effects of early poverty on cortisol in young children: moderation by autonomic nervous system activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38(11):2666–75.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Blair C, Berry D, Mills-Koonce R, Granger D, Investigators FLP. Cumulative effects of early poverty on cortisol in young children: moderation by autonomic nervous system activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38(11):2666–75.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
107.
Zurück zum Zitat Chen E, Cohen S, Miller GE. How low socioeconomic status affects 2-year hormonal trajectories in children. Psychol Sci. 2010;21(1):31–7.PubMedCrossRef Chen E, Cohen S, Miller GE. How low socioeconomic status affects 2-year hormonal trajectories in children. Psychol Sci. 2010;21(1):31–7.PubMedCrossRef
108.
Zurück zum Zitat Lumeng JC, Miller A, Peterson KE, Kaciroti N, Sturza J, Rosenblum K, et al. Diurnal cortisol pattern, eating behaviors and overweight in low-income preschool-aged children. Appetite. 2014;73:65–72.PubMedCrossRef Lumeng JC, Miller A, Peterson KE, Kaciroti N, Sturza J, Rosenblum K, et al. Diurnal cortisol pattern, eating behaviors and overweight in low-income preschool-aged children. Appetite. 2014;73:65–72.PubMedCrossRef
109.
Zurück zum Zitat Miller AL, Lumeng CN, Delproposto J, Florek B, Wendorf K, Lumeng JC. Obesity-related hormones in low-income preschool-age children: implications for school readiness. Mind Brain Educ. 2013;7(4):246–55.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Miller AL, Lumeng CN, Delproposto J, Florek B, Wendorf K, Lumeng JC. Obesity-related hormones in low-income preschool-age children: implications for school readiness. Mind Brain Educ. 2013;7(4):246–55.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
110.
Zurück zum Zitat O'Brien KM, Tronick E, Moore CL. Relationship between hair cortisol and perceived chronic stress in a diverse sample. Stress Health. 2013;29(4):337–44.PubMed O'Brien KM, Tronick E, Moore CL. Relationship between hair cortisol and perceived chronic stress in a diverse sample. Stress Health. 2013;29(4):337–44.PubMed
111.
Zurück zum Zitat Schreier HM, Roy LB, Frimer LT, Chen E. Family chaos and adolescent inflammatory profiles: the moderating role of socioeconomic status. Psychosom Med. 2014;76(6):460–7.PubMedCrossRef Schreier HM, Roy LB, Frimer LT, Chen E. Family chaos and adolescent inflammatory profiles: the moderating role of socioeconomic status. Psychosom Med. 2014;76(6):460–7.PubMedCrossRef
112.
Zurück zum Zitat Doom JR, Cook SH, Sturza J, Kaciroti N, Gearhardt AN, Vazquez DM, et al. Family conflict, chaos, and negative life events predict cortisol activity in low-income children. Dev Psychobiol. 2018;60(4):364–79.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Doom JR, Cook SH, Sturza J, Kaciroti N, Gearhardt AN, Vazquez DM, et al. Family conflict, chaos, and negative life events predict cortisol activity in low-income children. Dev Psychobiol. 2018;60(4):364–79.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
113.
Zurück zum Zitat Appelhans BM, Fitzpatrick SL, Li H, Cail V, Waring ME, Schneider KL, et al. The home environment and childhood obesity in low-income households: indirect effects via sleep duration and screen time. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:1160.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Appelhans BM, Fitzpatrick SL, Li H, Cail V, Waring ME, Schneider KL, et al. The home environment and childhood obesity in low-income households: indirect effects via sleep duration and screen time. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:1160.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
114.
Zurück zum Zitat Chae M, Taylor B, Lawrence J, Healey D, Reith D, Gray A, et al. Family CHAOS is associated with glycaemic control in children and adolescents with type 1 diabetes mellitus. Acta Diabetol. 2016;53(1):49–55.PubMedCrossRef Chae M, Taylor B, Lawrence J, Healey D, Reith D, Gray A, et al. Family CHAOS is associated with glycaemic control in children and adolescents with type 1 diabetes mellitus. Acta Diabetol. 2016;53(1):49–55.PubMedCrossRef
115.
Zurück zum Zitat Dush CM, Schmeer KK, Taylor M. Chaos as a social determinant of child health: reciprocal associations? Soc Sci Med. 2013;95:69–76.CrossRef Dush CM, Schmeer KK, Taylor M. Chaos as a social determinant of child health: reciprocal associations? Soc Sci Med. 2013;95:69–76.CrossRef
116.
Zurück zum Zitat Levin L, Kichler JC, Polfuss M. The relationship between hemoglobin A1C in youth with type 1 diabetes and chaos in the family household. Diabetes Educ. 2013;39(5):696–704.PubMedCrossRef Levin L, Kichler JC, Polfuss M. The relationship between hemoglobin A1C in youth with type 1 diabetes and chaos in the family household. Diabetes Educ. 2013;39(5):696–704.PubMedCrossRef
117.
Zurück zum Zitat Kraft SJ, Ambrose N, Chon H, editors. Temperament and environmental contributions to stuttering severity in children: the role of effortful control. New York: Seminars in speech and language: Thieme Medical Publishers; 2014. Kraft SJ, Ambrose N, Chon H, editors. Temperament and environmental contributions to stuttering severity in children: the role of effortful control. New York: Seminars in speech and language: Thieme Medical Publishers; 2014.
118.
Zurück zum Zitat Khatiwada A, Shoaibi A, Neelon B, Emond J, Benjamin-Neelon S. Household chaos during infancy and infant weight status at 12 months. Pediatr Obes. 2018;13:607–13. Khatiwada A, Shoaibi A, Neelon B, Emond J, Benjamin-Neelon S. Household chaos during infancy and infant weight status at 12 months. Pediatr Obes. 2018;13:607–13.
119.
Zurück zum Zitat Asta K, Miller AL, Retzloff L, Rosenblum K, Kaciroti NA, Lumeng JC. Eating in the absence of hunger and weight gain in low-income toddlers. Pediatrics. 2016;137(5):1–8.CrossRef Asta K, Miller AL, Retzloff L, Rosenblum K, Kaciroti NA, Lumeng JC. Eating in the absence of hunger and weight gain in low-income toddlers. Pediatrics. 2016;137(5):1–8.CrossRef
120.
Zurück zum Zitat Goulding AN, Lumeng JC, Rosenblum KL, Chen YP, Kaciroti N, Miller AL. Maternal feeding goals described by low-income mothers. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2015;47(4):331–7.e1.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Goulding AN, Lumeng JC, Rosenblum KL, Chen YP, Kaciroti N, Miller AL. Maternal feeding goals described by low-income mothers. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2015;47(4):331–7.e1.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
121.
Zurück zum Zitat Bartel K, Williamson P, van Maanen A, Cassoff J, Meijer AM, Oort F, et al. Protective and risk factors associated with adolescent sleep: findings from Australia, Canada, and The Netherlands. Sleep Med. 2016;26:97–103.PubMedCrossRef Bartel K, Williamson P, van Maanen A, Cassoff J, Meijer AM, Oort F, et al. Protective and risk factors associated with adolescent sleep: findings from Australia, Canada, and The Netherlands. Sleep Med. 2016;26:97–103.PubMedCrossRef
122.
Zurück zum Zitat Billows M, Gradisar M, Dohnt H, Johnston A, McCappin S, Hudson J. Family disorganization, sleep hygiene, and adolescent sleep disturbance. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2009;38(5):745–52.PubMedCrossRef Billows M, Gradisar M, Dohnt H, Johnston A, McCappin S, Hudson J. Family disorganization, sleep hygiene, and adolescent sleep disturbance. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2009;38(5):745–52.PubMedCrossRef
123.
Zurück zum Zitat Brown DD, Weatherholt TN, Burns BM. Attention skills and looking to television in children from low income families. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2010;31(4):330–8.CrossRef Brown DD, Weatherholt TN, Burns BM. Attention skills and looking to television in children from low income families. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2010;31(4):330–8.CrossRef
124.
Zurück zum Zitat Pesch MH, Wentz EE, Rosenblum KL, Appugliese DP, Miller AL, Lumeng JC. “You’ve got to settle down!”: Mothers’ perceptions of physical activity in their young children. BMC Pediatr. 2015;15:149.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Pesch MH, Wentz EE, Rosenblum KL, Appugliese DP, Miller AL, Lumeng JC. “You’ve got to settle down!”: Mothers’ perceptions of physical activity in their young children. BMC Pediatr. 2015;15:149.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
125.
Zurück zum Zitat Bronfenbrenner U, Morris PA. The bioecological model of human development. Handbook of child psychology; 2006. Bronfenbrenner U, Morris PA. The bioecological model of human development. Handbook of child psychology; 2006.
126.
Zurück zum Zitat Evans GW, Wachs TD. Chaos and its influence on children’s development. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2010. Evans GW, Wachs TD. Chaos and its influence on children’s development. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2010.
127.
Zurück zum Zitat Crandall A, Deater-Deckard K, Riley AW. Maternal emotion and cognitive control capacities and parenting: a conceptual framework. Dev Rev. 2015;36:105–26.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Crandall A, Deater-Deckard K, Riley AW. Maternal emotion and cognitive control capacities and parenting: a conceptual framework. Dev Rev. 2015;36:105–26.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
128.
129.
Zurück zum Zitat Shonkoff JP, Garner AS, Siegel BS, Dobbins MI, Earls MF, McGuinn L, et al. The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics. 2012;129(1):e232–e46.PubMedCrossRef Shonkoff JP, Garner AS, Siegel BS, Dobbins MI, Earls MF, McGuinn L, et al. The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics. 2012;129(1):e232–e46.PubMedCrossRef
130.
Zurück zum Zitat Kuras YI, Assaf N, Thoma MV, Gianferante D, Hanlin L, Chen X, et al. Blunted diurnal cortisol activity in healthy adults with childhood adversity. Front Hum Neurosci. 2017;11:574.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kuras YI, Assaf N, Thoma MV, Gianferante D, Hanlin L, Chen X, et al. Blunted diurnal cortisol activity in healthy adults with childhood adversity. Front Hum Neurosci. 2017;11:574.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
131.
Zurück zum Zitat Heim C, Newport DJ, Heit S, Graham YP, Wilcox M, Bonsall R, et al. Pituitary-adrenal and autonomic responses to stress in women after sexual and physical abuse in childhood. JAMA. 2000;284(5):592–7.PubMedCrossRef Heim C, Newport DJ, Heit S, Graham YP, Wilcox M, Bonsall R, et al. Pituitary-adrenal and autonomic responses to stress in women after sexual and physical abuse in childhood. JAMA. 2000;284(5):592–7.PubMedCrossRef
132.
Zurück zum Zitat Evans GW, Kliewer W, Martin J. The role of the physical environment in the health and well-being of children. New directions in health psychology assessment; 1991. p. 127–57. Evans GW, Kliewer W, Martin J. The role of the physical environment in the health and well-being of children. New directions in health psychology assessment; 1991. p. 127–57.
133.
Zurück zum Zitat Kildare CA, Middlemiss W. Impact of parents mobile device use on parent-child interaction: a literature review. Comput Hum Behav. 2017;75:579–93.CrossRef Kildare CA, Middlemiss W. Impact of parents mobile device use on parent-child interaction: a literature review. Comput Hum Behav. 2017;75:579–93.CrossRef
134.
Zurück zum Zitat Emond JA, Tantum LK, Gilbert-Diamond D, Kim SJ, Lansigan RK, Neelon SB. Household chaos and screen media use among preschool-aged children: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2018;18(1):1210.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Emond JA, Tantum LK, Gilbert-Diamond D, Kim SJ, Lansigan RK, Neelon SB. Household chaos and screen media use among preschool-aged children: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2018;18(1):1210.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
135.
Zurück zum Zitat Fullerton HN Jr. Labor force participation: 75 years of change, 1950-98 and 1998-2025. Monthly Lab Rev. 1999;122:3. Fullerton HN Jr. Labor force participation: 75 years of change, 1950-98 and 1998-2025. Monthly Lab Rev. 1999;122:3.
136.
Zurück zum Zitat Baxter J. Child care participation and maternal employment trends in Australia. Melbourne: Australian Institute of Family Studies; 2013. Baxter J. Child care participation and maternal employment trends in Australia. Melbourne: Australian Institute of Family Studies; 2013.
137.
Zurück zum Zitat Neumark-Sztainer D, Hannan PJ, Story M, Croll J, Perry C. Family meal patterns: associations with sociodemographic characteristics and improved dietary intake among adolescents. J Am Diet Assoc. 2003;103(3):317–22.PubMedCrossRef Neumark-Sztainer D, Hannan PJ, Story M, Croll J, Perry C. Family meal patterns: associations with sociodemographic characteristics and improved dietary intake among adolescents. J Am Diet Assoc. 2003;103(3):317–22.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
The relationship between household chaos and child, parent, and family outcomes: a systematic scoping review
verfasst von
Samantha Marsh
Rosie Dobson
Ralph Maddison
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2020
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Public Health / Ausgabe 1/2020
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2458
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-020-08587-8

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2020

BMC Public Health 1/2020 Zur Ausgabe