Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Public Health 1/2022

Open Access 01.12.2022 | Research

Maternal stress, child behavior and the promotive role of older siblings

verfasst von: Federica Amici, Stefan Röder, Wieland Kiess, Michael Borte, Ana C. Zenclussen, Anja Widdig, Gunda Herberth

Erschienen in: BMC Public Health | Ausgabe 1/2022

Abstract

Background

In the first years of their lives, children develop the cognitive, social and emotional skills that will provide the foundations for their lifelong health and achievements. To increase their life prospects and reduce the long-term effects of early aversive conditions, it is therefore crucial to understand the risk factors that negatively affect child development and the factors that are instead beneficial. In this study, we tested (i) the effects of different social and environmental stressors on maternal stress levels, (ii) the dynamic relationship between maternal stress and child behavior problems during development, and (iii) the potential promotive (i.e. main) or protective (i.e. buffering) effect of siblings on child behavior problems during development.

Methods

We used longitudinal data from 373 mother–child pairs (188 daughters, 185 sons) from pregnancy until 10 years of age. We assessed maternal stress and child behavior problems (internalizing and externalizing) with validated questionnaires, and then used linear mixed models, generalized linear mixed models and longitudinal cross-lagged models to analyze the data.

Results

Our results showed that higher maternal stress levels were predicted by socio-environmental stressors (i.e. the lack of sufficient social areas in the neighborhood). Moreover, prenatal maternal stress reliably predicted the occurrence of behavior problems during childhood. Finally, the presence of older siblings had a promotive function, by reducing the likelihood that children developed externalizing problems.

Conclusions

Overall, our results confirm the negative effects that maternal stress during pregnancy may have on the offspring, and suggest an important main effect of older siblings in promoting a positive child development.
Begleitmaterial
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s12889-022-13261-2.
Anja Widdig and Gunda Herberth contributed equally.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

In the first years of their lives, children develop the cognitive, social and emotional skills that will provide the foundations for their lifelong health and achievements [1]. In order to increase child life prospects and reduce the long-term effects of early aversive conditions, it is utterly necessary to understand both the risk factors that may negatively affect their healthy development and the factors that may instead promote or protect it [14]. According to the developmental origins of health and disease theory, exposure to environmental stressors during critical periods of life can have negative long-term consequences for children’s health and development [57]. Parental stress (i.e. maternal or paternal stress), for instance, which is caused by a variety of social and environmental factors, can have serious short- and long-term effects on children [8, 9], by increasing their risk of developing diseases and behavior problems (e.g. [1012]). Families, however, can have a promotive (i.e. main) and protective (i.e. buffering) function against the negative effects of stress on child development. Positive sibling relationships, for instance, may reduce the occurrence of behavior problems in children [13, 14], or mitigate the negative consequences of stressful events that children experience [15, 16]. In this study, we therefore explored (i) the effects of different socio-environmental stressors on maternal stress levels, (ii) the dynamic relationship between maternal stress (including prenatal stress) and child behavior problems during development, and (iii) the potential promotive or protective function of siblings toward the emergence of child behavior problems.

Socio-environmental factors contributing to maternal stress

Among the most studied risk factors for child health and development are those linked to parental stress [8, 9]. Depending on the characteristics of the stressors (e.g. their intensity, duration or predictability), and on the individual life histories (e.g. early exposure to stress [17, 18]), stressors can trigger adaptive and/or maladaptive responses, allowing individuals to optimally respond to potential threats and/or triggering the disruption of stress coping mechanisms, respectively [19]. When stressors are too intense, or too long, individuals may not manage to cope with them, i.e. the stress response system may remain activated and there can be important negative consequences on individual health [20].
Several stressors can activate the parental stress response system [2126], including acute disasters (e.g. terroristic attacks or droughts), psychological and physiological changes linked to maternity, work-related stress and even physical and emotional challenges experienced during everyday life [1, 21, 22, 27]. Parents, for instance, show higher levels of stress when they receive little social support, when their offspring experiences health or behavior problems, in case of conflict among family members, or in the presence of enduring socio-environmental stressors, like deprived environments [2326]. Individuals who are satisfied with the environment they live in, in contrast, may experience lower stress levels: a safe neighborhood characterized by high environmental quality (e.g. in terms of air quality), the presence of social areas, meeting places, shops and other infrastructures, for example, may all contribute to increase neighborhood satisfaction and well-being [2830] and hence decrease individual stress levels [31]. Similarly, individual stress levels may be lower when the quality of social relationships with neighbors is high, as neighbors can contribute to social integration and individual well-being (e.g. [32, 33]).

Prenatal stress as a long-term risk factor for child development

When parents are stressed, children have a higher risk of developing social, emotional, behavior and cognitive problems [8, 9, 3436]. Maternal stress can have a negative impact on offspring development already in the utero [3739], and even before conception [40]. Prenatal stress may have adverse effects on fetal development through an increase of stress hormones during pregnancy [41, 42], placental inflammation [43, 44], or by altering epigenetic regulation through changes in mRNA expression and DNA methylation [45, 46]. The consequences for children can be substantial. Prenatal maternal stress can modify the fetal immune system [47], affect birth outcome [48, 49] and lead to several diseases during childhood, like obesity or wheezing [50, 51].
Prenatal stress can also have negative effects on offspring psychosocial development [5254], as well as brain and cognitive development [52, 5557]. Moreover, stress during pregnancy might increase the likelihood of behavior problems during childhood [54, 58], including attention deficit or hyperactivity [53, 59], conduct disorders [53, 60], and even increase the occurrence of autism [61], depression and schizophrenia in children [62]. Prenatal stress, for instance, reliably predicts the occurrence of internalizing (e.g. anxiety, poor self-estimation) and/or externalizing (e.g. hyperactivity, aggression) behavior problems in children (e.g. [12, 58, 6368]), even when accounting for maternal stress levels after pregnancy (e.g. [69, 70]. These findings are not restricted to extreme prenatal stress or response to acute disasters (e.g. terroristic attacks or droughts), but they may also extend to maternal stress caused by daily challenges, pregnancy anxiety or relationship strain [21, 22].
Maternal stress can also be linked to the occurrence of offspring behavior problems in the short term. Several studies have shown a clear link between the occurrence of parental stress and children’s internalizing and externalizing behavior problems (e.g. [7175]). When mothers suffer from stress, for instance, children consistently show an increasing risk of developing internalizing problems, like depression and anxiety, and externalizing problems, like aggressive and rule-breaking behavior [75].
The relationship between maternal stress and child behavior problems, however, is complex, as parents and children reciprocally affect each other. Whereas children are more likely to develop behavior, social or emotional problems if their parents suffer from stress, children with these problems may in turn increase parental stress, giving rise to a cycle of negative parent–child interactions with adverse outcomes for all parties involved [8, 34, 7679]. Longitudinal studies and methodological approaches (e.g. cross-lagged models) that assess how parental stress and child behavior problems reciprocally affect each other are therefore necessary to disentangle to what extent maternal stress contributes to child behavior problems, and/or the other way round, and/or whether other external factors can explain the link between these two variables (see e.g. [8]).

The role of siblings as protective or promotive factor

Among the most important environmental factors that affect child development are parental and family well-being (e.g., [8082]). On the one hand, the family environment can entail risk factors for the child development, including parental stress (see above). On the other hand, families have an essential promotive or protective function for children’s healthy development, in the face of aversive conditions. Although there is no consensus in literature, promotive factors are usually considered those factors that have direct ameliorative effects on child development, being linked to a general decrease in behavior problems (i.e. as main effect), whereas protective factors usually indicate those factors that buffer children from the occurrence of behavior problems in the face of risk (i.e. in interaction with other factors; [4]). A healthy social environment, for instance, can provide increased social support to parents, directly reducing their stress levels and increasing their psychological well-being (i.e. promotive function), or it can have an indirect function by mitigating the negative consequences of parental stress on children (i.e. protective function) [83, 84].
To date, several studies have shown that siblings can significantly contribute to family well-being. Siblings are an essential part of family systems [85], they usually have long-lasting relationships with each other and spend abundant time together [8688]. Therefore, siblings may learn to understand each other’s thoughts, emotions and intentions from early on, facilitating the development of social, emotional and cognitive skills [8991]. Moreover, siblings may serve an important promotive or protective function during child development, reducing the occurrence of child behavior problems and/or mitigating the negative consequences of stressful events [1316]. By generally contributing to a healthy social and cognitive development, positive sibling relationships may indeed reduce the likelihood that children show adjustment problems (e.g. [87]; for reviews, see [13, 14]). Children with siblings, for instance, are less likely to develop internalizing problems, even when controlling for maternal stress levels [75].
Moreover, siblings may exert a protective function for children, by reducing the risk that adverse conditions trigger the emergence of internalizing and externalizing problems [15, 16]. Positive sibling relationships, for example, can decrease the probability that children develop internalizing problems after experiencing stressful events [15]. However, the mechanisms beyond these processes are not clear. Siblings, especially older ones, may provide each other comfort and security, or they may more successfully distract each other when experiencing stress [15]. Either way, siblings may have a protective function in case of adverse conditions, increasing their ability to positively react to environmental stressors [15, 92].
Although longitudinal studies are especially important to further explore the promotive and protective function of siblings and temporally disentangle the complex interplay of parental stress and child behavior problems, such an approach has rarely been used [15]. However, it can be crucial to understand how parental stress and child behavior problems are linked to each other through time, and whether the presence of siblings provides direct or indirect benefits to children during their development. Moreover, siblings can have a different role depending on their age and gender. While older siblings are usually more prone to engage in caregiving and helping roles (and their protective/promotive function may thus be especially relevant), younger siblings are usually more likely to elicit help and care (see [91]). Furthermore, some studies have shown that older sisters are more likely to engage in caretaking and helping behavior toward siblings, as compared to older brothers [93, 94]. Therefore, separately assessing the role of older brother and sisters may be essential to understand the protective and/or promotive function that siblings may have during child development.

The present study

In this study, we used longitudinal data on maternal stress levels and child behavior to test (i) the effects of different social and environmental stressors on maternal stress levels, (ii) the dynamic relationship between maternal stress and child behavior problems during development, and (iii) the potential promotive/protective function of siblings during child development. In this way, we aimed to contribute novel findings on the complex interplay of socio-environmental stressors, maternal stress and child behavior problems, by especially focusing on the role of promotive/protective factors in this relationship. Moreover, we aimed to confirm previous findings linking maternal stress and child behavior problems—an important endeavor in psychology to increase reproducibility (e.g. [95]). Based on previous studies (see above), we made the following predictions. First, we predicted that maternal stress (which we assessed when children were aged 10) would be higher when mothers are less satisfied with the environment they live in, in terms of living standards (e.g. air quality, safety) and quality of social relationships with neighbors. Second, we predicted that higher levels of maternal stress would be linked to an increased probability of internalizing (e.g. anxiety, poor self-estimation) and/or externalizing (e.g. hyperactivity, aggression) behavior problems in children. In particular, we predicted that prenatal maternal stress would have a negative long-term effect on offspring behavior during childhood (which we assessed when children were 7, 8 and 10), whereas maternal stress and behavior problems (both assessed when children were 7, 8 and 10) would be linked to each other throughout development. As maternal stress and behavior problems during childhood may reciprocally affect each other, we further used longitudinal cross-lagged models to better disentangle the link between these variables. Finally, we predicted that the presence of older siblings would exert a protective or promotive function for children [15, 16], by buffering the negative effects of maternal stress on child behavior or by overall reducing the occurrence of behavior problems in children through their development (i.e. when children were 7, 8 and 10).

Methods

Participants

This study was based on the prospective mother–child cohort LINA (Lifestyle and environmental factors and their Influence on Newborns Allergy risk) and included longitudinal data on maternal stress levels and child behavior from pregnancy until the age of 10 years of children life. We initially screened the data for 629 mother–child pairs recruited between March 2006 and December 2008 in Leipzig, Germany, among Caucasian mothers who did not suffer from severe infectious or immune diseases during pregnancy [96]. In the analyses presented here, we only included a LINA sub-cohort of 373 mother–child pairs for which information was available on both child behavior and maternal stress levels (see below; Fig. 1). For an overview of the main characteristics of the LINA sub-cohort analyzed in this study, including measures of socio-economic status, please refer to Table S1 in Suppl. Material. Participants were assessed longitudinally with standardized questionnaires (see below) self-administered by the parents, which measured (i) maternal stress levels when the mother was pregnant (i.e. 34th-36th week of gestation), and (ii) both maternal stress and child behavior in three waves (i.e. when the child was 7, 8 and 10 years of age; Fig. 1). All mothers signed an informed consent.

Maternal stress

We assessed maternal stress using a 20-item perceived stress questionnaire (PSQ [97, 98]). The PSQ is a validated questionnaire commonly used to assess how often (on a 4-point scale) mothers experience stress (e.g. worries, loss of joy, tension). For each questionnaire, we summed all the scored answers to each item (from 0 = almost never to 3 = mostly) and then divided the sum for the maximum score that could be obtained (i.e., 60), in order to obtain individual indices of maternal stress ranging from 0 to 1 (i.e., from less to more stressed; see [98]). Maternal stress was assessed longitudinally, first during pregnancy, and then in the three waves (i.e. when the child was 7, 8 and 10 years of age; Fig. 1). Internal consistency for this questionnaire was high, with Cronbach’s alpha ranging from 0.92 to 0.94 in the three waves.

Child behavior: internalizing and externalizing behavior problems

Mothers were asked to assess their children’s behavior in the previous six months, at the three waves (i.e. when the child was 7, 8 and 10 years of age), using the standardized 25-item Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) that assesses internalizing and externalizing problems, including emotional, mental and behavioral ones [99]. More specifically, the SDQ questionnaire contained five questions for each of the following five dimensions: (i) hyperactivity/lack of attention (e.g. how easily the child is distracted), (ii) emotional symptoms (e.g. how often the child is unhappy or tearful), (iii) conduct problems (e.g. how often the child has temper tantrums), (iv) peer relationship problems (i.e. how solitary the child is), and (v) prosocial behaviors (i.e. how helpful the child is when others are hurt or upset). Given that only the first four dimensions refer to behavior problems [99], we first aggregated the four dimensions by summing the answers to the respective items that were assessed using a response format from 0 (not true) to 2 (true), obtaining a total difficulties score that ranged between 0 and 40. For modelling purposes (i.e. to model dependent variables as proportions, varying from 0 to 1), we then divided this score by the maximum score that could be obtained (i.e., 40; see [99]), so that the individual indices ranged from 0 to 1. Internal consistency for this questionnaire was only moderate (with Cronbach’s alpha ranging from 0.53 to 0.62 in the three waves), as expected given that the questionnaire includes different dimensions [99].

Child behavior: externalizing behavior problems

We further asked mothers to assess child behavior in the three waves (i.e. when the child was 7, 8 and 10 years of age), using an adapted version of the Fremdbeurteilungsbogen für hyperkinetische Störungen (FBB-HKS), which provides a 20-item external assessment of child hyperkinetic disorders [100], and thus a more specific focus on children’s externalizing problems. Mothers were provided with 18 questions (see supplementary material for the list of questions) to be assessed on a 4-point scale (e.g. how often the child seems not to be listening when being talked to). As above, we calculated a second behavioral index ranging from 0 to 1 (i.e. from less to more problematic), by summing the scored answers (from 0 to 3, with 3 always indicating higher behavior problems) and then dividing the sum for the maximum score that could be obtained (i.e. 54). Internal consistency for this questionnaire was high, with Cronbach’s alpha ranging from 0.90 to 0.91 in the three waves. Behavior problems as assessed with the SDQ and the FBB-HKS questionnaires correlated in the three waves (Spearman exact test: p < 0.001, N = 251, rho in the first wave = 0.677, in the second wave: 0.746, and in the third wave: 0.742).

Other information

In the three waves (i.e. when children were aged 7, 8 and 10), mothers were also asked for information on the number of siblings at home, their sex and age, and their own education level (on a 7-point scale, from pre-primary education to second stage of tertiary education; see [101]). In the third wave (i.e. when children were 10), we also used subjective measures to assess the potential impact of socio-environmental stressors, by asking mothers how satisfied they were with the environment they lived in, using 16 questions on a 4-point scale (i.e. with 0 meaning low satisfaction, and 3 high satisfaction). We included questions on mothers’ evaluation of their living standards, including (i) natural environment (e.g. air quality), (ii) safety of the area, (iii) presence of social areas and meeting places (e.g. playgrounds), (iv) shops and (v) other infrastructures (e.g. public transport). We thus calculated 5 indexes ranging from 0 to 1 (i.e. from lower to higher satisfaction), by summing the relative scored answers and dividing the sum for the maximum score that could be obtained. We further included 5 questions on the quality of social relationships with the neighbors, 2 on a 4-point scale (e.g. how good is the relationship to the neighbors) and 3 as polar questions (e.g. whether there are friends in the area, who are regularly met). As above, we calculated an index from 0 to 1 (i.e. from lower to higher satisfaction), by summing the scored answers and dividing the sum for the maximum score that could be obtained (i.e. 9). Therefore, these indexes were higher when mothers rated themselves as happier with their environment, and lower when they were less satisfied with it. Finally, mothers in the third wave (i.e. when children were 10) also provided information on the monthly family income (as a categorical variable from 0 to 9, with 0 being a lower income and 9 a higher one).

Statistical analyses

We used linear mixed models, generalized linear mixed models and longitudinal cross-lagged models to analyze the data. For this purpose, we prepared three data-sets, (i) to study the factors predicting maternal stress levels (Model 1); (ii) to assess the link between maternal stress (i.e. prenatal and postnatal) and child behavior problems, and the potential promotive or protective effect of siblings on child behavior problems (Models 2 and 3); and (iii) to study the dynamic relationship between maternal stress and behavior problems during child development (i.e. in the three waves; longitudinal cross-lagged models). In the first data-set we entered one line for each child (i.e. only including the third wave; N = 268; Fig. 1). We then entered the individual indices for maternal stress levels in the third wave, maternal satisfaction (i.e. with neighbors, natural environment, safety of the area, presence of social areas, shops and other infrastructures), family income (as low income has been associated to increased stress, e.g. [102]), maternal education level (as education may have a protective function against stress, e.g. [103]) and number of children in the household (as the presence of more children at home can increase parental stress levels, e.g. [104]). In the second data-set, we entered one line for each child and wave (i.e. up to three lines per child, as tested at 7, 8 and 10 years of age; Fig. 1). As some children were not tested at all ages, we ended up with N = 974 lines in this data-set. We then entered child identity, SDQ and FBB-HKS behavioral indexes, maternal stress as assessed (i) prenatally and (ii) in the three waves, the number of siblings living in the same household (i.e. the overall number, the number of older sisters and the number of older brothers), the child’s gender (because males were more likely be reported to show behavioral problems, e.g. [105]), the child’s age (because the occurrence of behavioral problems may vary through development), and maternal education level (as this may have a positive impact on child development, e.g. [106, 107]). Further controls (e.g. family income) could not be added in this data-set, as this information was not available for all waves. In the third data-set, we entered one line for each child (N = 373), including maternal stress levels and SDQ and FBB-HKS behavioral indexes for each of the three waves, and further specifying whether the child had older siblings. We never aggregated data of the three waves.
Data were analyzed using R (version 3.5.0 [108]). We first used the glmmTMB package (version 1.0.1 [109]) to build three linear mixed models and generalized linear mixed models (Models 1 to 3; Fig. 1). In order to test for the role of social and environmental factors on maternal stress levels, we run the first linear mixed model with the first data-set, using maternal stress in the third wave as the dependent variable (Model 1). As predictors, we included several measures of maternal satisfaction with the environment (i.e. relationship quality with neighbors, natural environment, safety of the area, presence of social areas, shops and other infrastructures). We further controlled for the number of children in the household and for family income (which is known to reliably predict parental stress [110]).
In order to test for the link between maternal stress and child behavior problems, as well as the potential protective effect of siblings, we ran two generalized linear mixed models with the second data-set, using the SDQ (Model 2) and FBB-HKS (Model 3) behavioral indexes in the three waves, respectively, as dependent variables (Fig. 1). In both models, we included prenatal maternal stress and maternal stress in the wave as test predictors. To assess the protective role of siblings as social buffers against maternal stress, we further included as predictor the three 2-way interactions of maternal stress in the three waves with i) the overall number of siblings at home, ii) the number of older brothers and iii) the number of older sisters, respectively, and their main terms. If the 2-way interactions were non-significant, the interaction was removed and the model was re-run by including only the single predictors, thus allowing us to test for the promotive role of siblings on child behavior problems. We also included children’s sex as predictor, as sex may also predict behavior problems in children [111]. Finally, we controlled for children’s age (as our data were collected in three different waves), and included child identity as random factor to control for lack of independence, as the same children were entered multiple times in the data-set.
As the dependent variables in Models 1 to 3 were modelled as proportions including 0 and 1, we used a beta distribution after transforming them [112]. We also z-transformed continuous test predictors (i.e. age) to facilitate convergence. We compared full models to null models (i.e. only containing control predictors and random factors) using likelihood ratio tests [113]. When full and null models significantly differed, we obtained the p values for each test predictor using the R function summary. We detected no over-dispersion, convergence or stability issues in our models. There was also no problem of collinearity in our models (maximum VIFs across all models = 2.08).
In order to test for the dynamic relationship between changes in maternal stress and changes in child behavior problems during development, we run a further set of models, using the third dataset and the lavaan package (version 4.0 [114]) to build longitudinal cross-lagged models [115] (Fig. 1). These longitudinal models allow to disentangle the interplay of two variables across waves, and thus better infer possible causal pathways linking the two variables. In particular, it is possible to assess both cross domain-relationships (i.e. the predictive effect of one variable on the other one as tested in the following wave) and autocorrelations (i.e. the predictive effect of one variable on the same variable as tested in the following wave). In these longitudinal models, we always used full information maximum likelihood for missing data to reduce bias and increase statistical power [115]. We compared models of increasing complexity (see below) using likelihood ratio tests and also reported fit indices for the individual models, including robust Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), robust Comparative Fit Index (CFI), robust Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) and Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR).
To run our longitudinal cross-lagged models, we first constructed child behavior problems as a latent variable, measured by a set of two observed measurements (i.e. SDQ and FBB-HKS behavioral indexes). We established measurement invariance through time by using a multiple indicator univariate model. In particular, we established configural, metric and scalar invariance by sequentially constraining factor loadings, error terms and intercepts through time [115]. All models fit the data well (configural model with no parameter constraints: χ2 = 19.28, df = 3, RMSEA = 0.130, CFI = 0.985, TLI = 0.925, SRMR = 0.024; metric model with factor loading and error term invariance: χ2 = 20.09, df = 11, RMSEA = 0.066, CFI = 0.986, TLI = 0.980, SRMR = 0.037; scalar model with intercept invariance: χ2 = 24.00, df = 13, RMSEA = 0.064, CFI = 0.984, TLI = 0.982, SRMR = 0.038). Crucially, there were no significant differences between models with increasing constraints (configural-metric model comparison: χ2 = 7.61, df = 8, p = 0.473; metric-scalar model comparison: χ2 = 4.23, df = 2, p = 0.121), suggesting scalar measurement invariance for child behavior problems through time.
After establishing measurement invariance, we tested for possible relationships between changes of maternal stress and changes in child behavior problems during the three waves (i.e. at 7, 8 and 10 years of age), using a longitudinal cross-lagged model. We decomposed each of the two variables into its score in the preceding wave and its change from the preceding to the current wave [115]. We then tested all possible relationships between maternal stress and child behavior problems: (i) stress-behavior covariance in the first wave, (ii) stress to behavior coupling, to assess whether stress scores in one wave predicted rates of changes in behavioral scores, (iii) behavior to stress coupling, to test whether behavioral scores in one wave predicted rates of changes in stress scores, and (iv) estimates of correlated changes, to assess whether stress-behavior changes co-occur after accounting for the coupling pathways (see [115] for details). In the model, we also specified an auto-regression parameter for maternal stress and another one for child behavior problems, to measure how change in each variable depended on the scores in the preceding wave. To obtain finer-graded measures of these relationships, and given the low number of waves in our model, we only assessed changes between adjacent waves [116]. Finally, we tested whether the relationships between maternal stress and child behavior problems differed for children with or without older siblings, and assessed whether this more complex model provided an increase of fit as compared to the preceding one [115].

Results

With regards to their behavior problems as scored with the SDQ, children in our study were in the 45th percentile at age 7, and in the 44th percentile at age 8 and 10, as compared to normative data for the German population [117, 118]. In particular, whereas they scored above the 50th percentile for emotional symptoms in all waves (54th-59th), they always scored below the 50th percentile for all the other dimensions (hyperactivity/lack of attention: 42nd-47th; conduct problems: 42nd-43th; peer relationship problems: 36th-40th). Moreover, most mothers (69% on average) reported a low incidence of behavior problems in their children (i.e. SDQ indices between 0 and 0.25; at 7 years: 70%, at 8 years: 69%, at 10 years: 68%). Further 26% of mothers reported an intermediate incidence of behavior problems (i.e. 0.25 ≤ SDQ indices < 0.50; 26% across all age classes). The minority of mothers reported either no child behavior problems (at 7 years: 2%; at 8 years: 3%; at 10 years: 3%), or frequent problems (i.e. SDQ ≥ 0.50; at 7 years: 2%; at 8 years: 2%; at 10 years: 4%). Figure 2 summarizes the mean indices for maternal stress levels (as assessed with the PSQ questionnaire) and for child behavior problems (as assessed with the SDQ and FBB-HKS questionnaires) in the three waves. Perceived maternal stress increased across child development. On average (± SD), mothers reported a PSQ index of 0.31 (± 0.16) during pregnancy, which increased to 0.43 (± 0.19) in the first wave (i.e. when the child was 7), 0.41 (± 0.19) in the second wave (i.e. when the child was 8) and 0.42 (± 0.19) in the third wave (i.e. when the child was 10). Therefore, maternal stress was significantly higher in the first, second and third wave, as compared to prenatal stress levels (exact Wilcoxon test for prenatal vs. the first wave: N = 235, z = -9.261, p < 0.001; vs. the second wave: N = 227, z = -7.879, p < 0.001; vs. the third wave: N = 229, z = -8.620, p < 0.001; Fig. 2).
In Model 1, we analyzed the effects of different social and environmental stressors on maternal stress levels. We detected a significant difference between the full and the null model (χ2 = 22.14, df = 6, p = 0.001; Table 1). In particular, the probability of reported maternal stress was higher when mothers were not satisfied with the presence of social areas in the place they lived (p = 0.017, est = -0.72, se = 0.30, z = -2.39; Fig. S1), while no other test predictor had a significant effect in our study.
Table 1
For each model, estimates, standard errors (SE), confidence intervals (CIs), z and p values of test and control predictors
MODEL
Estimate
SE
2.5% CI
97.5% CI
z values
P
M1: Probability of reporting maternal stress in the PSQ questionnaire in the 3rd wave
 Intercept
1.00
0.34
0.33
1.66
2.92
0.004
 Relationship with neighbors
-0.48
0.28
-1.02
0.07
-1.70
0.090
 Natural environment
0.12
0.34
-0.54
0.78
0.36
0.722
 Safety of the area
-0.16
0.33
-0.80
0.48
-0.48
0.630
 Availability of social areas
-0.72
0.30
-1.31
-0.13
-2.39
0.017*
 Availability of shops
-0.15
0.25
-0.65
0.34
-0.61
0.542
 Availability of other infrastructures
-0.41
0.37
-1.13
0.32
-1.10
0.272
Maternal education
0.02
0.05
-0.08
0.12
0.36
0.717
Family monthly income
-0.03
0.02
-0.08
0.01
-1.36
0.174
Number of children in household
-0.05
0.06
-0.18
0.07
-0.87
0.382
M2: Probability of reporting child behavior problems in the SDQ questionnaire in the 3 waves
 Intercept
-1.97
0.19
-2.35
-1.59
-10.12
 < 0.001
 Child gender (ref. category: female)
0.26
0.07
0.12
0.41
3.51
 < 0.001 *
 Maternal stress (prenatal)
0.78
0.26
0.27
1.28
3.03
0.002 *
 Maternal stress (in the 3 waves)
0.72
0.18
0.38
1.07
4.09
 < 0.001 *
 Number of siblings
0.02
0.05
-0.07
0.11
0.35
0.728
 Number of older sisters
-0.10
0.10
-0.29
0.09
-0.99
0.324
 Number of older brothers
-0.12
0.10
-0.31
0.07
-1.25
0.211
Maternal education
-0.07
0.04
-0.15
0.00
-1.87
0.061
Child age
-0.01
0.02
-0.05
0.02
-0.73
0.464
M3: Probability of reporting child behavior problems in the FBB-HKS questionnaire in the 3 waves
 Intercept
-2.42
0.25
-2.92
-1.92
-9.52
 < 0.001
 Child gender (ref. category: female)
0.47
0.10
0.28
0.67
4.84
 < 0.001*
 Maternal stress (prenatal)
0.82
0.33
0.16
1.47
2.45
0.014*
 Maternal stress (in the 3 waves)
0.82
0.21
0.42
1.23
3.98
 < 0.001*
 Number of siblings
0.10
0.05
-0.01
0.20
1.79
0.073
 Number of older sisters
-0.24
0.12
-0.47
-0.01
-2.01
0.044*
 Number of older brothers
-0.42
0.12
-0.65
-0.19
-3.60
 < 0.001*
Maternal education
-0.05
0.05
-0.15
0.06
-0.91
0.365
Child age
-0.01
0.02
-0.05
0.02
-0.71
0.477
In Models 2 and 3, we tested the link between maternal stress and child behavior problems, and the potential protective effect of older siblings. For Model 2, we found a significant difference between the full and the null model (χ2 = 58.33, df = 9, p < 0.001; Table 1). In particular, the probability of reporting child behavior problems in the SDQ questionnaire was higher for male children (p < 0.001, est = 0.26, se = 0.07, z = 3.51), when mothers reported higher prenatal stress (p = 0.002, est = 0.78, se = 0.26, z = 3.03; Fig. S2a) and when they were more stressed in that wave (p < 0.001, est = 0.72, se = 0.18, z = 4.09; Fig. S2b).
The full and the null model also significantly differed for Model 3 (χ2 = 74.17, df = 9, p < 0.001; Table 1). The probability of reporting child behavior problems in the FBB-HKS questionnaire was higher for male children (p < 0.001, est = 0.47, se = 0.10, z = 4.84), when mothers reported higher prenatal stress (p = 0.014, est = 0.82, se = 0.33, z = 2.45; Fig. S2c) and when mothers reported higher stress in that wave (p < 0.001, est = 0.82, se = 0.21, z = 3.98; Fig. S2d). In addition, the presence of older brothers (p < 0.001, est = -0.42, se = 0.12, z = -3.60) and older sisters (p = 0.044, est = -0.24, se = 0.12, z = -2.01) was linked to a lower probability of reporting child behavior problems, regardless of maternal stress levels, suggesting a promotive (i.e. main) effect of older siblings.
The longitudinal cross-lagged model in Fig. 3 allowed us to assess the dynamic relationship between maternal stress and child behavior problems during development. The model had a good fit (χ2 = 44.18, df = 24, RMSEA = 0.056, CFI = 0.985, TLI = 0.976, SRMR = 0.032), which was significantly better than the one of the corresponding univariate model (χ2 = 20.12, df = 11, p = 0.044). Variances in the cross-lagged model showed significant inter-individual differences in maternal stress and child behavior in the first wave (both p < 0.001), and in their rate of change between wave 1 and 2 (both p ≤ 0.002), and between wave 2 and 3 (both p ≤ 0.001; Table S2 in Suppl. Material). Intercept values showed significant mean level changes in maternal stress (but not in child behavior), with stress increasing from wave 1 to wave 2 (p = 0.013; est = 0.05, se = 0.02, z = 2.48) and from wave 2 to wave 3 (p < 0.001; est = 0.09, se = 0.02, z = 4.00; Table S2 in Suppl. Material).
Inspection of the four parameters linking maternal stress and child behavior across waves showed no significant cross-domain relationships (stress to behavior coupling in waves 2 and 3: p = 0.093 and p = 0.415; behavior to stress coupling in waves 2 and 3: p = 0.260 and p = 0.969; estimate of correlated change in waves 2 and 3: p = 0.115 and p = 0.118), except for stress-behavior covariance in the first wave (p < 0.001; est = 0.01, se = 0.00, z = 5.98; Fig. 3; Table S2 in Suppl. Material). In other words, higher maternal stress was linked to higher behavior problems in children at 7 years of age, and although maternal stress and child behavior problems were always significantly correlated in the three waves (Spearman exact test: all p < 0.001, N = 228, with rho ranging from 0.229 to 0.402), these relationships failed to reach significance after taking into account the other dynamic parameters (see non-significant estimates of correlated change in Table S2 in Suppl. Material). Furthermore, auto-regression parameters for both maternal stress and child behavior showed significant autocorrelation of stress and behavior. In particular, changes in maternal stress between waves 1 and 2 depended on maternal stress in the first wave (p = 0.044; est = -0.11, se = 0.05, z = -2.01), and changes in maternal stress between 2 and 3 depended on maternal stress in the second wave (p < 0.001; est = -0.20, se = 0.05, z = -3.83; Fig. 3; Table S2 in Suppl. Material). Moreover, changes in child behavior between waves 1 and 2 depended on child behavior in the first wave (p = 0.033; est = -0.13, se = 0.06, z = -2.13), although this relationship was not significant between waves 2 and 3 (p = 0.231; Fig. 3; Table S2 in Suppl. Material). Finally, we tested whether these relationships between maternal stress and child behavior differed for children with or without older siblings by using a multi-group model. Although the multi-group model had a good fit (χ2 = 66.23, df = 49, RMSEA = 0.049, CFI = 0.988, TLI = 0.985, SRMR = 0.036), including the effect of siblings did not provide a significant increase of fit as compared to the less complex cross-lagged model from Fig. 32 = 19.41, df = 25, p = 0.777). Therefore, we retained the latter model and refrained from further comparisons between children with and without older siblings.

Discussion

In this study, we used longitudinal data from 373 mother–child pairs to investigate the complex link between socio-environmental stressors, maternal stress and child behavior problems, in order to detect potential risk factors and protective or promotive factors. First, we assessed whether maternal satisfaction with the environment they lived in (in terms of living standards and quality of social relationships with neighbors) predicted maternal stress levels. Second, we tested whether prenatal stress predicted the occurrence of behavior problems during childhood, and how maternal stress and child behavior problems dynamically affected each other during development. Finally, we assessed the role of siblings as a protective factor buffering the negative effects of maternal stress on child behavior, or generally promoting a healthy child development.
Partially in line with our predictions, mothers reported higher levels of perceived stress when they were not satisfied with the quality of the environment they lived in (Model 1). In particular, the lack of sufficient social areas in the neighborhood (including playgrounds, meeting venues for young people and cultural activities) significantly predicted higher levels of maternal stress. In contrast, other potential stressors (e.g. poor quality relationship with neighbors, low quality of the natural environment, low safety, lack of infrastructures) did not significantly predict maternal stress levels in our study. Incidentally, having a higher number of children also did not predict higher maternal stress levels (Table 1). Previous research has shown a consistent link between environmental quality (e.g. air pollution, lack of green spaces) and individual stress levels [119, 120]. Access to green areas, for instance, is known to buffer the relation between income-related deprivation and mortality in other countries [120]. In our study, however, it was the presence of social meeting places and cultural offers in the neighborhood (rather than the quality of the natural environment) that mostly exerted a positive impact on maternal stress levels. Our study was conducted in Leipzig, which is a relatively small city (about 600,000 inhabitants) with a high proportion of greenness (i.e. on average, 16 m2 green space per inhabitant [121]. Therefore, it is possible that the quality of the natural environment is generally high compared to other areas, and the relative importance of social areas on maternal stress may be more important.
In line with our predictions, higher levels of maternal stress were linked to an increased probability of behavior problems in children. In particular, prenatal maternal stress predicted the occurrence of behavior problems during childhood, as measured with both the SDQ and the FBB-HKS questionnaires (Models 2 and 3). These results confirm the long-term negative effect of prenatal stress on child behavior [53, 54, 5860], which can happen through a variety of mechanisms, from placental signaling to changes in inflammatory and epigenetic pathways (for a review, see [122]). Our results held true in all three waves (i.e. when children were 7, 8 and 10 years), and thus confirm that the negative effects of prenatal stress might last at least into late childhood [56, 58]. This is consistent with previous studies suggesting that prenatal stress, through epigenetic mechanisms or stress hormones directly affecting the placenta and the fetus [45, 46] might have a uniquely powerful impact on child development. Therefore, our study confirms the importance of prevention of prenatal stress and early intervention to improve maternal health and well-being already during pregnancy, in order to reduce the risks of maternal stress and its negative consequences on child development.
In our study, the use of the SDQ and FBB-HKS questionnaires led to similar results, in that prenatal stress predicted the occurrence of behavior problems during childhood when using both the SDQ questionnaire (Model 2) and the FBB-HKS questionnaire (Model 3). These two questionnaires partly measure different behavior problems. In particular, while the SDQ questionnaire assesses both internalizing and externalizing problems (i.e. including behavior problems that span from hyperactivity/lack of attention to emotional symptoms, conduct problems and peer relationship problems [99]), the FBB-HKS questionnaire is more strictly focused on the occurrence of externalizing problems like child hyperkinetic disorders [100]. Therefore, these results suggest that prenatal stress may have a negative impact on a wide range of behavior problems, from hyperkinetic disorders to emotional symptoms, conduct problems and peer relationship problems. In the future, it would be interesting to further investigate whether maternal stress is linked to specific domains of the SDQ questionnaire (e.g. emotional symptoms rather than conduct problems), as internal consistency in this study suggest important variation across these domains.
Also in line with our predictions, higher maternal stress in the tree waves predicted a higher probability of reporting behavior problems in children, as assessed by both the SDQ (Model 2) and the FBB-HKS questionnaires (Model 3). To disentangle whether high maternal stress levels triggered the onset of child behavior problems, or the other way round, or if other external factors explained the link between child behavior and maternal stress, we used longitudinal models. Previous studies have suggested that contingent parental stress may increase the risk that children develop behavioral and cognitive problems [3436]. In turn, the occurrence of behavior problems in children would increase parental stress [23, 123], triggering a cycle of negative parent–child interactions [8, 34, 76, 77]. Our longitudinal models, however, did not replicate these findings and showed no clear evidence of cross-domain-relationships, except for a significant covariance of maternal stress and child behavior in the first wave. Given that both measures of maternal stress and child behavior problems were based on maternal reports (rather than more objective assessments), caution is needed even when interpreting this cross-domain relationship (e.g. being more stressed, mothers might have wrongly perceived their children as showing more behavior problems). In contrast, we found an interesting autocorrelation effect for both maternal stress and child behavior, with higher maternal stress levels predicting a decrease in maternal stress in the following wave, and more behavior problems in the first wave also predicting a decrease in behavior problems in the second wave. These results suggest a self-containment effect of stress and behavior problems, with a tendency to return to average levels, perhaps because parents may search for help or take more effective measures when reaching critical stress levels or when detecting high behavior problems in their children.
In line with our predictions on the promotive role of siblings as generally promoting a healthy child development, mothers were overall more likely to report child behavior problems in the absence of older brothers and sisters, independently of their stress levels and child’s gender. Therefore, our results suggest that the presence of older siblings overall reduced the likelihood that younger siblings developed behavior problems during childhood, rather than acting as social buffers in case of maternal stress (i.e. the presence of older siblings had a significant main effect in Model 3). This effect was however limited to Model 3, in which child behavior problems were assessed through the FBB-HKS questionnaire (i.e. focusing on externalizing problems such as hyperkinetic disorders [100]), and did not extend to Model 2, in which child behavior problems were instead assessed through the SDQ questionnaire (i.e. including both internalizing and externalizing problems [99]). Therefore, it is possible that the promotive function of older siblings may be limited to externalizing problems.
Interestingly, the presence of older siblings was a significant negative predictor of child behavior problems in Model 3, but the interaction between contingent maternal stress levels and presence of older siblings was not significant, neither in Model 2 nor in Model 3. Therefore, the presence of older siblings overall reduced the likelihood that children developed behavior problems during childhood, but did not specifically mitigate maternal stress. Hence, we agree with authors suggesting an important promotive function of older siblings (see [13, 14, 87]). These results are confirmed by the lack of a significant increase of fit when using multi-group models (see the non-significant comparison between the multi-group and the cross-lagged models). This suggests that the presence of older siblings, while being generally linked to a lower occurrence of child behavior problems (Model 3), failed to modulate the negative effects of maternal stress (as instead found by Gass and colleagues [15]). The direct link between presence of older siblings and lower incidence of child behavior problems may be easily explained with existing literature. By interacting with their older siblings, children may develop better emotional, perspective taking and problem solving skills [123, 124], which are linked to higher social competence and emotion understanding also later in life [125, 126]. Moreover, it is possible that the presence of older siblings provided learning opportunities for parents, affecting their expectations and behavior toward younger offspring (see [14]). Parents with previous experience with adolescent offspring, for instance, are less likely to expect behavior problems during adolescence in younger children [127]. Therefore, a lower incidence of behavior problems in children with older siblings may also depend on different expectations and better parental skills [128].
Finally, mothers were more likely to report child behavior problems for male offspring, with both questionnaires (Models 2 and 3). Our results are not surprising, as sons have often been reported to show more behavior problems than daughters, especially in terms of hyperactivity and lack of attention [101, 129]. In our opinion, however, these results should warn us against the risk that current questionnaires on child behavior might inappropriately categorize male behavior as being problematic (or even suggestive of serious behavioral disorders), simply because the needs of children (both boys and girls) are unlikely to be satisfied in modern Western cultures.
The main strength of our study is the availability and use of longitudinal data from mother–child pairs with several follow-ups from birth until the age of 10. However, in the future, it would be interesting to include biological measures of individual stress rather than self-reported ones and better differentiate between different forms of stress (e.g. acute, episodic, chronic), in order to specifically investigate the promotive and/or protective function of siblings and other members of the social network during child development. Moreover, it would be important to better understand how the role of siblings and other family members differs in different cultural contexts, as there is substantial variation in the way sibling relationships are valued across cultures [130]. In this regard, it would be especially interesting to include measures that specifically assess the quality of sibling relationships, while still using a longitudinal approach, as this allows to better disentangle the complex interplay of parental stress, child behavior problems and several other factors through time. Furthermore, our study sample was unfortunately limited, in that it only included mother–child pairs living in Germany. However, there may be important cross-cultural differences in the link between maternal stress, the occurrence of child behavior problems and the promotive role of older siblings. The promotive function of older siblings, for instance, may be even stronger in cultures where siblings spend more time with each other, or in populations at increased risk of negative stress responses, like families with lower family income or education level [102, 103]. Finally, considering that small household sizes with only one child have become increasingly common in several countries (e.g. in Europe, North-America, China [131]), future studies should particularly focus on how the lack of siblings may affect child development and, especially, on the identification of other promotive factors that may avoid the emergence of internalizing and externalizing problems in children, even in the absence of siblings.
Overall, our study largely confirms previous research on the relationship between social and environmental stressors, maternal stress levels and child behavior, and thus contributes in highlighting the importance of long-term policies of prevention of prenatal stress and early intervention, aimed to increase maternal well-being and reduce the risks of maternal stress already during pregnancy. In particular, this study emphasizes the important role that siblings may have in the family to increase the likelihood of a healthy development for children, and calls for public health policies that directly target children and their siblings, and aim to promote their general well-being and a healthy environment for the development of high-quality sibling relationships.

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge support from the Max Planck Society for the opportunity to publish Open Access.

Declarations

This study was carried out in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki and complied with APA ethical standards. It was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Leipzig and the Saxonian Board of Physicians (046–2006, 160–2008, 160b/2008, 144–10-31052010, 113–11-18042011, 206–12-02072012, 169/13-ff, 150/14-ff, EK-allg-28/14–1, 008/17-ek). Informed consent was obtained from all study subjects (i.e. mothers).
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Anhänge

Supplementary Information

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Shonkoff JP, Garner AS. The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics. 2012;129:232–46.CrossRef Shonkoff JP, Garner AS. The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics. 2012;129:232–46.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Austin AE, Gottfredson NC, Halpern CT, Zolotor AJ, Marshall SW, Parrish JW, Shanahan ME. Patterns of risk and protective factors among Alaska children: association with maternal and child well-being. Chld Dev. 2020;91:1650–62.CrossRef Austin AE, Gottfredson NC, Halpern CT, Zolotor AJ, Marshall SW, Parrish JW, Shanahan ME. Patterns of risk and protective factors among Alaska children: association with maternal and child well-being. Chld Dev. 2020;91:1650–62.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Garmezy N, Masten AS, Tellegen A. The study of stress and competence in children: a building block for developmental psychopathology. Child Dev. 1984;55:97–111.PubMedCrossRef Garmezy N, Masten AS, Tellegen A. The study of stress and competence in children: a building block for developmental psychopathology. Child Dev. 1984;55:97–111.PubMedCrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Brumley LD, Jaffee SR. Defining and distinguishing promotive and protective effects for childhood externalizing psychopathology: a systematic review. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2016;51:803–15.PubMedCrossRef Brumley LD, Jaffee SR. Defining and distinguishing promotive and protective effects for childhood externalizing psychopathology: a systematic review. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2016;51:803–15.PubMedCrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Barker DJ. The origins of the developmental origins theory. J Intern Med. 2007;261(5):412–7.PubMedCrossRef Barker DJ. The origins of the developmental origins theory. J Intern Med. 2007;261(5):412–7.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Glover V, O’Connor TG, O’Donnell K. Prenatal stress and the programming of the HPA axis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;35:17–22.PubMedCrossRef Glover V, O’Connor TG, O’Donnell K. Prenatal stress and the programming of the HPA axis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;35:17–22.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Crnic K, Low C. Everyday stresses and parenting. In: Bornstein MH, editor. Handbook of parenting: practical issues in parenting. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2002. p. 243–67. Crnic K, Low C. Everyday stresses and parenting. In: Bornstein MH, editor. Handbook of parenting: practical issues in parenting. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2002. p. 243–67.
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Deater-Deckard K, Panneton R, editors. Parental stress and early child development. Springer; 2017. Deater-Deckard K, Panneton R, editors. Parental stress and early child development. Springer; 2017.
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Kingsbury M, Weeks M, MacKinnon N, Evans J, Mahedy L, Dykxhoorn J, Colman I. Stressful life events during pregnancy and offspring depression: evidence from a prospective cohort study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;55:709–16.PubMedCrossRef Kingsbury M, Weeks M, MacKinnon N, Evans J, Mahedy L, Dykxhoorn J, Colman I. Stressful life events during pregnancy and offspring depression: evidence from a prospective cohort study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;55:709–16.PubMedCrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat MacKinnon N, Kingsbury M, Mahedy L, Evans J, Colman I. The association between prenatal stress and externalizing symptoms in childhood: evidence from the avon longitudinal study of parents and children. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;83:100–8.PubMedCrossRef MacKinnon N, Kingsbury M, Mahedy L, Evans J, Colman I. The association between prenatal stress and externalizing symptoms in childhood: evidence from the avon longitudinal study of parents and children. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;83:100–8.PubMedCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Van den Bergh BRH, van den Heuvel MI, Lahti M, Braeken M, de Rooij SR, Entringer S, Hoyer D, Roseboom T, Räikkönen K, King S, Schwab M. Prenatal developmental origins of behavior and mental health: the influence of maternal stress in pregnancy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020;117:26–64.PubMedCrossRef Van den Bergh BRH, van den Heuvel MI, Lahti M, Braeken M, de Rooij SR, Entringer S, Hoyer D, Roseboom T, Räikkönen K, King S, Schwab M. Prenatal developmental origins of behavior and mental health: the influence of maternal stress in pregnancy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020;117:26–64.PubMedCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Feinberg ME, Solmeyer AR, McHale SM. The third rail of family systems: sibling relationships, mental and behavioral health, and preventive intervention in childhood and adolescence. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2012;15:43–57.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Feinberg ME, Solmeyer AR, McHale SM. The third rail of family systems: sibling relationships, mental and behavioral health, and preventive intervention in childhood and adolescence. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2012;15:43–57.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat McHale SM, Updegraff KA, Whiteman SD. Sibling relationships and influences in childhood and adolescence. J Marriage Family. 2012;74:913–30.CrossRef McHale SM, Updegraff KA, Whiteman SD. Sibling relationships and influences in childhood and adolescence. J Marriage Family. 2012;74:913–30.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Gass K, Jenkins J, Dunn J. Are sibling relationships protective? A longitudinal study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48:167–75.PubMedCrossRef Gass K, Jenkins J, Dunn J. Are sibling relationships protective? A longitudinal study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48:167–75.PubMedCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Peltonen K, Qouta S, El Sarraj E, Punamäki RL. Military trauma and social development: the moderating and mediating roles of peer and sibling relations in mental health. Int J Behav Dev. 2010;34:554–63.CrossRef Peltonen K, Qouta S, El Sarraj E, Punamäki RL. Military trauma and social development: the moderating and mediating roles of peer and sibling relations in mental health. Int J Behav Dev. 2010;34:554–63.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:434–45.PubMedCrossRef Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:434–45.PubMedCrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat McEwen BS. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiol Rev. 2007;87:873–904.PubMedCrossRef McEwen BS. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiol Rev. 2007;87:873–904.PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Suri D, Vaidya VA. The adaptive and maladaptive continuum of stress responses - a hippocampal perspective. Rev Neurosci. 2015;26:415–42.PubMedCrossRef Suri D, Vaidya VA. The adaptive and maladaptive continuum of stress responses - a hippocampal perspective. Rev Neurosci. 2015;26:415–42.PubMedCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Mariotti A. The effects of chronic stress on health: new insights into the molecular mechanisms of brain-body communication. Future Sci OA. 2015;1:FSO23.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mariotti A. The effects of chronic stress on health: new insights into the molecular mechanisms of brain-body communication. Future Sci OA. 2015;1:FSO23.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Davis EP, Hankin BL, Glynn LM, Head K, Kim DJ, Sandman CA. Prenatal maternal stress, child cortical thickness, and adolescent depressive symptoms. Child Dev. 2020;91:432–50.CrossRef Davis EP, Hankin BL, Glynn LM, Head K, Kim DJ, Sandman CA. Prenatal maternal stress, child cortical thickness, and adolescent depressive symptoms. Child Dev. 2020;91:432–50.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Glover V. The effects of prenatal stress on child behavioral and cognitive outcomes start at the beginning encyclopedia on early childhood development. 2011. Glover V. The effects of prenatal stress on child behavioral and cognitive outcomes start at the beginning encyclopedia on early childhood development. 2011.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Nelson SK, Kushlev K, Lyubomirsky S. The pains and pleasures of parenting: when, why, and how is parenthood associated with more or less well-being? Psychol Bull. 2014;140:846–95.PubMedCrossRef Nelson SK, Kushlev K, Lyubomirsky S. The pains and pleasures of parenting: when, why, and how is parenthood associated with more or less well-being? Psychol Bull. 2014;140:846–95.PubMedCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Evans GW, Kim P. Childhood poverty, chronic stress, self-regulation, and coping. Child Dev Persp. 2013;7:43–8.CrossRef Evans GW, Kim P. Childhood poverty, chronic stress, self-regulation, and coping. Child Dev Persp. 2013;7:43–8.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Hair NL, Hanson JL, Wolfe BL, Pollak SD. Association of child poverty, brain development, and academic achievement. JAMA Pediatr. 2015;169:822–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hair NL, Hanson JL, Wolfe BL, Pollak SD. Association of child poverty, brain development, and academic achievement. JAMA Pediatr. 2015;169:822–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Miller GE, Chen E. The biological residue of childhood poverty. Child Dev Persp. 2013;7:67–73.CrossRef Miller GE, Chen E. The biological residue of childhood poverty. Child Dev Persp. 2013;7:67–73.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Razurel C, Kaiser B, Sellenet C, Epiney M. Relation between perceived stress, social support, and coping strategies and maternal well-being: a review of the literature. Women Health. 2013;53:74–99.PubMedCrossRef Razurel C, Kaiser B, Sellenet C, Epiney M. Relation between perceived stress, social support, and coping strategies and maternal well-being: a review of the literature. Women Health. 2013;53:74–99.PubMedCrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Mouratidis K. Neighborhood characteristics, neighborhood satisfaction, and well-being: the links with neighborhood deprivation. Land Use Policy. 2020;99:104886.CrossRef Mouratidis K. Neighborhood characteristics, neighborhood satisfaction, and well-being: the links with neighborhood deprivation. Land Use Policy. 2020;99:104886.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Macintyre S, MacIver S, Sooman A. Area, class and health: should we be focusing on places or people? J Soc Policy. 1993;22:213–34.CrossRef Macintyre S, MacIver S, Sooman A. Area, class and health: should we be focusing on places or people? J Soc Policy. 1993;22:213–34.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Rogerson RJ, Findlay AM, Morris AS. Indicators of quality of life: Some methodological issues. Environ Plan. 1989;21:1655–66.CrossRef Rogerson RJ, Findlay AM, Morris AS. Indicators of quality of life: Some methodological issues. Environ Plan. 1989;21:1655–66.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Steptoe A, Feldman PJ. Neighborhood problems as sources of chronic stress: development of a measure of neighborhood problems, and associations with socioeconomic status and health. Ann Behav Med. 2001;23:177–85.PubMedCrossRef Steptoe A, Feldman PJ. Neighborhood problems as sources of chronic stress: development of a measure of neighborhood problems, and associations with socioeconomic status and health. Ann Behav Med. 2001;23:177–85.PubMedCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Skoner DP, Rabin BS, Gwaltney JM. Social ties and susceptibility to the common cold. JAMA. 1997;277:1940–4.PubMedCrossRef Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Skoner DP, Rabin BS, Gwaltney JM. Social ties and susceptibility to the common cold. JAMA. 1997;277:1940–4.PubMedCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Baker CE, Brooks-Gunn J, Goiskova N. Reciprocal relations between maternal depression and child behavior problems in families served by head start. Child Dev. 2020;91:1563–76.PubMedCrossRef Baker CE, Brooks-Gunn J, Goiskova N. Reciprocal relations between maternal depression and child behavior problems in families served by head start. Child Dev. 2020;91:1563–76.PubMedCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Cummings E, Keller PS, Davies PT. Towards a family process model of maternal and paternal depressive symptoms: exploring multiple relations with child and family functioning. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46:479–89.PubMedCrossRef Cummings E, Keller PS, Davies PT. Towards a family process model of maternal and paternal depressive symptoms: exploring multiple relations with child and family functioning. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46:479–89.PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Weitzman M, Rosenthal DG, Liu YH. Paternal depressive symptoms and child behavioral or emotional problems in the United States. Pediatrics. 2011;128:1126–34.PubMedCrossRef Weitzman M, Rosenthal DG, Liu YH. Paternal depressive symptoms and child behavioral or emotional problems in the United States. Pediatrics. 2011;128:1126–34.PubMedCrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Bussieres EL, Tarabulsy GM, Pearson J, Tessier R, Forest JC, Giguère Y. Maternal prenatal stress and infant birth weight and gestational age: a meta-analysis of prospective studies. Dev Rev. 2015;36:179–99.CrossRef Bussieres EL, Tarabulsy GM, Pearson J, Tessier R, Forest JC, Giguère Y. Maternal prenatal stress and infant birth weight and gestational age: a meta-analysis of prospective studies. Dev Rev. 2015;36:179–99.CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Grote NK, Bridge JA, Gavin AR, Melville JL, Iyengar S, Katon WJ. A meta-analysis of depression during pregnancy and the risk of preterm birth, low birth weight, and intrauterine growth restriction. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:1012–24.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Grote NK, Bridge JA, Gavin AR, Melville JL, Iyengar S, Katon WJ. A meta-analysis of depression during pregnancy and the risk of preterm birth, low birth weight, and intrauterine growth restriction. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:1012–24.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Kingston D, Tough S, Whitfield H. Prenatal and postpartum maternal psychological distress and infant development: a systematic review. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2012;43:683–714.PubMedCrossRef Kingston D, Tough S, Whitfield H. Prenatal and postpartum maternal psychological distress and infant development: a systematic review. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2012;43:683–714.PubMedCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Spry E, Moreno-Betancur M, Becker D, Romaniuk H, Carlin JB, Molyneaux EFL, et al. Maternal mental health and infant emotional reactivity: a 20-year two-cohort study of preconception and perinatal exposures. Psychol Med. 2019;50:827–37.PubMedCrossRef Spry E, Moreno-Betancur M, Becker D, Romaniuk H, Carlin JB, Molyneaux EFL, et al. Maternal mental health and infant emotional reactivity: a 20-year two-cohort study of preconception and perinatal exposures. Psychol Med. 2019;50:827–37.PubMedCrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Kapoor A, Dunn E, Kostaki A, Andrews MH, Matthews SG. Fetal programming of hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal function: prenatal stress and glucocorticoids. J Physiol. 2006;572:31–44.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kapoor A, Dunn E, Kostaki A, Andrews MH, Matthews SG. Fetal programming of hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal function: prenatal stress and glucocorticoids. J Physiol. 2006;572:31–44.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Sandman CA, Davis EP, Buss C, Glynn LM. Exposure to prenatal psychobiological stress exerts programming influences on the mother and her fetus. Neuroendocrinology. 2012;95:7–21.PubMedCrossRef Sandman CA, Davis EP, Buss C, Glynn LM. Exposure to prenatal psychobiological stress exerts programming influences on the mother and her fetus. Neuroendocrinology. 2012;95:7–21.PubMedCrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Bronson SL, Bale TL. Prenatal stress-induced increases in placental inflammation and offspring hyperactivity are male-specific and ameliorated by maternal antiinflammatory treatment. Endocrinology. 2014;155:2635–46.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Bronson SL, Bale TL. Prenatal stress-induced increases in placental inflammation and offspring hyperactivity are male-specific and ameliorated by maternal antiinflammatory treatment. Endocrinology. 2014;155:2635–46.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Chen HJ, Antonson AM, Rajasekera TA, Patterson JM, Bailey MT, Gur TL. Prenatal stress causes intrauterine inflammation and serotonergic dysfunction, and long-term behavioral deficits through microbe- and CCL2-dependent mechanisms. Transl Psychiatry. 2020;10:191.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Chen HJ, Antonson AM, Rajasekera TA, Patterson JM, Bailey MT, Gur TL. Prenatal stress causes intrauterine inflammation and serotonergic dysfunction, and long-term behavioral deficits through microbe- and CCL2-dependent mechanisms. Transl Psychiatry. 2020;10:191.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Babenko O, Kovalchuk I, Metz GAS. Stress-induced perinatal and transgenerational epigenetic programming of brain development and mental health. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;48:70–91.PubMedCrossRef Babenko O, Kovalchuk I, Metz GAS. Stress-induced perinatal and transgenerational epigenetic programming of brain development and mental health. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;48:70–91.PubMedCrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Monk C, Spicer J, Champagne FA. Linking prenatal maternal adversity to developmental outcomes in infants: the role of epigenetic pathways. Dev Psychopathol. 2012;24:1361–76.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Monk C, Spicer J, Champagne FA. Linking prenatal maternal adversity to developmental outcomes in infants: the role of epigenetic pathways. Dev Psychopathol. 2012;24:1361–76.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Wright RJ. Prenatal maternal stress and early caregiving experiences: implications for childhood asthma risk. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2007;21:8–14.PubMedCrossRef Wright RJ. Prenatal maternal stress and early caregiving experiences: implications for childhood asthma risk. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2007;21:8–14.PubMedCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Hobel CJ, Goldstein A, Barrett ES. Psychosocial stress and pregnancy outcome. Clin Obstet Gynecol. 2008;51:333–48.PubMedCrossRef Hobel CJ, Goldstein A, Barrett ES. Psychosocial stress and pregnancy outcome. Clin Obstet Gynecol. 2008;51:333–48.PubMedCrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Walsh K, McCormack CA, Webster R, Pinto A, Lee S, Feng T, Krakovsky SH, O’Grady SM, Tycko B, Champagne FA, Werner EA, Liu G, Monk C. Maternal prenatal stress phenotypes associate with fetal neurodevelopment and birth outcomes. PNAS. 2019;116:23996–4005.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Walsh K, McCormack CA, Webster R, Pinto A, Lee S, Feng T, Krakovsky SH, O’Grady SM, Tycko B, Champagne FA, Werner EA, Liu G, Monk C. Maternal prenatal stress phenotypes associate with fetal neurodevelopment and birth outcomes. PNAS. 2019;116:23996–4005.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Dancause KN, Laplante DP, Hart KJ, O’Hara MW, Elgbeili G, Brunet A, King S. Prenatal stress due to a natural disaster predicts adiposity in childhood: the Iowa flood study. J Obes. 2015;2015:570541.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Dancause KN, Laplante DP, Hart KJ, O’Hara MW, Elgbeili G, Brunet A, King S. Prenatal stress due to a natural disaster predicts adiposity in childhood: the Iowa flood study. J Obes. 2015;2015:570541.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Trump S, Bieg M, Gu Z, Thürmann L, Bauer T, Bauer M, Ishaque N, Röder S, Gu L, Herberth G, Lawerenz C, Borte M, Schlesner M, Plass C, Diessl N, Eszlinger M, Mücke O, Elvers HD, Wissenbach DK, von Bergen M, Herrmann C, Weichenhan D, Wright RJ, Lehmann I, Eils R. Prenatal maternal stress and wheeze in children: novel insights into epigenetic regulation. Sci Rep. 2016;6:28616.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Trump S, Bieg M, Gu Z, Thürmann L, Bauer T, Bauer M, Ishaque N, Röder S, Gu L, Herberth G, Lawerenz C, Borte M, Schlesner M, Plass C, Diessl N, Eszlinger M, Mücke O, Elvers HD, Wissenbach DK, von Bergen M, Herrmann C, Weichenhan D, Wright RJ, Lehmann I, Eils R. Prenatal maternal stress and wheeze in children: novel insights into epigenetic regulation. Sci Rep. 2016;6:28616.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Bergman K, Sarkar P, O’Connor TG, Modi N, Glover V. Maternal stress during pregnancy predicts cognitive ability and fearfulness in infancy. JAAPEE. 2007;46:1454–63.PubMed Bergman K, Sarkar P, O’Connor TG, Modi N, Glover V. Maternal stress during pregnancy predicts cognitive ability and fearfulness in infancy. JAAPEE. 2007;46:1454–63.PubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Connor TG, Heron J, Golding J, Glover V. Maternal antenatal anxiety and behavioral/emotional problems in children: a test of a programming hypothesis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44:1025–36.PubMedCrossRef O’Connor TG, Heron J, Golding J, Glover V. Maternal antenatal anxiety and behavioral/emotional problems in children: a test of a programming hypothesis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44:1025–36.PubMedCrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat van den Bergh BR, Marcoen A. High antenatal maternal anxiety is related to ADHD symptoms, externalizing problems, and anxiety in 8- and 9-year-olds. Child Dev. 2004;75:1085–97.PubMedCrossRef van den Bergh BR, Marcoen A. High antenatal maternal anxiety is related to ADHD symptoms, externalizing problems, and anxiety in 8- and 9-year-olds. Child Dev. 2004;75:1085–97.PubMedCrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Buss C, Davis EP, Muftuler LT, Head K, Sandman CA. High pregnancy anxiety during mid-gestation is associated with decreased gray matter density in 6-9-year-old children. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;35:141–53.CrossRef Buss C, Davis EP, Muftuler LT, Head K, Sandman CA. High pregnancy anxiety during mid-gestation is associated with decreased gray matter density in 6-9-year-old children. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;35:141–53.CrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Davis EP, Sandman CA. The timing of prenatal exposure to maternal cortisol and psychosocial stress is associated with human infant cognitive development. Child Dev. 2010;81:131–48.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Davis EP, Sandman CA. The timing of prenatal exposure to maternal cortisol and psychosocial stress is associated with human infant cognitive development. Child Dev. 2010;81:131–48.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Troller-Renfree SV, Brito NH, Desai PM, Leon-Santos A, Wiltshire CA, Motten SN, Meyer JS, Isler JR, Fifer WP, Noble KG. Infants of mothers with higher physiological stress show alterations in brain function. Dev Sci. 2020;23:e12976.PubMed Troller-Renfree SV, Brito NH, Desai PM, Leon-Santos A, Wiltshire CA, Motten SN, Meyer JS, Isler JR, Fifer WP, Noble KG. Infants of mothers with higher physiological stress show alterations in brain function. Dev Sci. 2020;23:e12976.PubMed
58.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Donnell KJ, Glover V, Barker ED, O’Connor TG. The persisting effect of maternal mood in pregnancy on childhood psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. 2014;26:393–403.PubMedCrossRef O’Donnell KJ, Glover V, Barker ED, O’Connor TG. The persisting effect of maternal mood in pregnancy on childhood psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. 2014;26:393–403.PubMedCrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Huizink AC, Dick DM, Sihvola E, Pulkkinen L, Rose RJ, Kaprio J. Chernobyl exposure as stressor during pregnancy and behavior in adolescent offspring. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2007;116:438–46.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Huizink AC, Dick DM, Sihvola E, Pulkkinen L, Rose RJ, Kaprio J. Chernobyl exposure as stressor during pregnancy and behavior in adolescent offspring. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2007;116:438–46.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Barker ED, Maughan B. Differentiating early-onset persistent versus childhood-limited conduct problem youth. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166:900–8.PubMedCrossRef Barker ED, Maughan B. Differentiating early-onset persistent versus childhood-limited conduct problem youth. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166:900–8.PubMedCrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Walder DJ, Laplante DP, Sousa-Pires A, Veru F, Brunet A, King S. Prenatal maternal stress predicts autism traits in 61/2 year-old children: project ice storm. Psychiatry Res. 2014;219:353–60.PubMedCrossRef Walder DJ, Laplante DP, Sousa-Pires A, Veru F, Brunet A, King S. Prenatal maternal stress predicts autism traits in 61/2 year-old children: project ice storm. Psychiatry Res. 2014;219:353–60.PubMedCrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Khashan AS, Abel KM, McNamee R, Pedersen MG, Webb RT, Baker PN, et al. Higher risk of offspring schizophrenia following antenatal maternal exposure to severe adverse life events. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:146–52.PubMedCrossRef Khashan AS, Abel KM, McNamee R, Pedersen MG, Webb RT, Baker PN, et al. Higher risk of offspring schizophrenia following antenatal maternal exposure to severe adverse life events. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:146–52.PubMedCrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Betts KS, Williams GM, Najman JM, Alati R. Maternal depressive, anxious, and stress symptoms during pregnancy predict internalizing problems in adolescence. Depress Anxiety. 2014;31:9–18.PubMedCrossRef Betts KS, Williams GM, Najman JM, Alati R. Maternal depressive, anxious, and stress symptoms during pregnancy predict internalizing problems in adolescence. Depress Anxiety. 2014;31:9–18.PubMedCrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Huizink AC, de Medina PG, Mulder EJ, Visser GH, Buitelaar JK. Psychological measures of prenatal stress as predictors of infant temperament. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatr. 2002;41:1078–85.CrossRef Huizink AC, de Medina PG, Mulder EJ, Visser GH, Buitelaar JK. Psychological measures of prenatal stress as predictors of infant temperament. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatr. 2002;41:1078–85.CrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Clayborne ZM, Nilsen W, Torvik FA, Gustavson K, Bekkhus M, Gilman SE, et al. Prenatal maternal stress, child internalizing and externalizing symptoms, and the moderating role of parenting: findings from the Norwegian mother, father, and child cohort study. Psychol Med. 2021;1–11. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721004311. Clayborne ZM, Nilsen W, Torvik FA, Gustavson K, Bekkhus M, Gilman SE, et al. Prenatal maternal stress, child internalizing and externalizing symptoms, and the moderating role of parenting: findings from the Norwegian mother, father, and child cohort study. Psychol Med. 2021;1–11. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1017/​S003329172100431​1.
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Gerardin P, Wendland J, Bodeau N, Galin A, Bialobos S, Tordjman S, Mazet P, Darbois Y, Nizard J, Dommergues M, Cohen D. Depression during pregnancy: is the developmental impact earlier in boys? A prospective case-control study. J Clin Psychiatry. 2011;72:378–87.PubMedCrossRef Gerardin P, Wendland J, Bodeau N, Galin A, Bialobos S, Tordjman S, Mazet P, Darbois Y, Nizard J, Dommergues M, Cohen D. Depression during pregnancy: is the developmental impact earlier in boys? A prospective case-control study. J Clin Psychiatry. 2011;72:378–87.PubMedCrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Leis JA, Heron J, Stuart EA, Mendelson T. Associations between maternal mental health and child emotional and behavioral problems: does prenatal mental health matter? J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2014;42:161–71.PubMedCrossRef Leis JA, Heron J, Stuart EA, Mendelson T. Associations between maternal mental health and child emotional and behavioral problems: does prenatal mental health matter? J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2014;42:161–71.PubMedCrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat van Batenburg-Eddes T, Brion MJ, Henrichs J, Jaddoe VW, Hofman A, Verhulst FC, Lawlor DA, Davey Smith G, Tiemeier H. Parental depressive and anxiety symptoms during pregnancy and attention problems in children: a cross-cohort consistency study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54:591–600.PubMedCrossRef van Batenburg-Eddes T, Brion MJ, Henrichs J, Jaddoe VW, Hofman A, Verhulst FC, Lawlor DA, Davey Smith G, Tiemeier H. Parental depressive and anxiety symptoms during pregnancy and attention problems in children: a cross-cohort consistency study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54:591–600.PubMedCrossRef
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Barker ED, Jaffee SR, Uher R, Maughan B. The contribution of prenatal and postnatal maternal anxiety and depression to child maladjustment. Depress Anxiety. 2011;28:696–702.PubMedCrossRef Barker ED, Jaffee SR, Uher R, Maughan B. The contribution of prenatal and postnatal maternal anxiety and depression to child maladjustment. Depress Anxiety. 2011;28:696–702.PubMedCrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Park S, Kim BN, Kim JW, Shin MS, Yoo HJ, Lee J, Cho SC. Associations between maternal stress during pregnancy and offspring internalizing and externalizing problems in childhood. Int J Ment Health Syst. 2014;8:44.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Park S, Kim BN, Kim JW, Shin MS, Yoo HJ, Lee J, Cho SC. Associations between maternal stress during pregnancy and offspring internalizing and externalizing problems in childhood. Int J Ment Health Syst. 2014;8:44.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Renk K, Roddenberry A, Oliveros A, Sieger K. The relationship of maternal characteristics and perceptions of children to children´s emotional and behavioral problems. Child Fam Behav Ther. 2007;29:37–57.CrossRef Renk K, Roddenberry A, Oliveros A, Sieger K. The relationship of maternal characteristics and perceptions of children to children´s emotional and behavioral problems. Child Fam Behav Ther. 2007;29:37–57.CrossRef
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Costa NM, Weems CF, Pellerin K, Dalton R. Parenting stress and childhood psychopathology: an examination of specificity to internalizing and externalizing symptoms. J PsychopatholBehavAssess. 2006;28:113–22. Costa NM, Weems CF, Pellerin K, Dalton R. Parenting stress and childhood psychopathology: an examination of specificity to internalizing and externalizing symptoms. J PsychopatholBehavAssess. 2006;28:113–22.
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Barry TD, Dunlap ST, Cotten SJ, Lochman JE, Wells KC. The influence of maternal stress and distress on disruptive behavior problems in boys. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;44:265–73.PubMedCrossRef Barry TD, Dunlap ST, Cotten SJ, Lochman JE, Wells KC. The influence of maternal stress and distress on disruptive behavior problems in boys. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;44:265–73.PubMedCrossRef
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Anthony LG, Anthony BJ, Glanville DN, Naiman DQ, Waanders C, Shaffer S. The relationships between parenting stress, parenting behavior and preschoolers’ social competence and behavior problems in the classroom. Infant Child Dev. 2005;14:133–54.CrossRef Anthony LG, Anthony BJ, Glanville DN, Naiman DQ, Waanders C, Shaffer S. The relationships between parenting stress, parenting behavior and preschoolers’ social competence and behavior problems in the classroom. Infant Child Dev. 2005;14:133–54.CrossRef
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Bayer JK, Hiscock H, Ukoumunne OC, Price A, Wake M. Early childhood aetiology of mental health problems: a longitudinal population-based study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49:1166–74.PubMed Bayer JK, Hiscock H, Ukoumunne OC, Price A, Wake M. Early childhood aetiology of mental health problems: a longitudinal population-based study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49:1166–74.PubMed
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Barroso NE, Mendez L, Graziano PA, Bagner DM. Parenting stress through the lens of different clinical groups: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2018;46:449–61.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Barroso NE, Mendez L, Graziano PA, Bagner DM. Parenting stress through the lens of different clinical groups: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2018;46:449–61.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Neece CL, Green SA, Baker BL. Parenting stress and child behavior problems: a transactional relationship across time. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2012;117:48–66.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Neece CL, Green SA, Baker BL. Parenting stress and child behavior problems: a transactional relationship across time. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2012;117:48–66.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Rodriguez CM. Association between independent reports of maternal parenting stress and children’s internalizing symptomatology. J Child Fam Stud. 2011;20:631–9.CrossRef Rodriguez CM. Association between independent reports of maternal parenting stress and children’s internalizing symptomatology. J Child Fam Stud. 2011;20:631–9.CrossRef
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Williford AP, Calkins SD, Keane SP. Predicting change in parenting stress across early childhood: child and maternal factors. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2007;35:251–63.PubMedCrossRef Williford AP, Calkins SD, Keane SP. Predicting change in parenting stress across early childhood: child and maternal factors. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2007;35:251–63.PubMedCrossRef
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Dix T. The affective organization of parenting: adaptive and maladaptive processes. Psychol Bull. 1991;110:3–25.PubMedCrossRef Dix T. The affective organization of parenting: adaptive and maladaptive processes. Psychol Bull. 1991;110:3–25.PubMedCrossRef
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Goodman SH, Rouse MH, Connell AM, Broth MR, Hall CM, Heyward D. Maternal depression and child psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2011;14:1–27.PubMedCrossRef Goodman SH, Rouse MH, Connell AM, Broth MR, Hall CM, Heyward D. Maternal depression and child psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2011;14:1–27.PubMedCrossRef
82.
Zurück zum Zitat NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Chronicity of maternal depressive symptoms, maternal sensitivity, and child functioning at 36 months. Dev Psychol. 1999;35:1297–310.CrossRef NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Chronicity of maternal depressive symptoms, maternal sensitivity, and child functioning at 36 months. Dev Psychol. 1999;35:1297–310.CrossRef
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Cohen S, Wills TA. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychol Bull. 1985;98:310–57.PubMedCrossRef Cohen S, Wills TA. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychol Bull. 1985;98:310–57.PubMedCrossRef
84.
Zurück zum Zitat Moak ZB, Agrawal A. The association between perceived interpersonal social support and physical and mental health. J Public Health. 2010;32:191–201.CrossRef Moak ZB, Agrawal A. The association between perceived interpersonal social support and physical and mental health. J Public Health. 2010;32:191–201.CrossRef
85.
Zurück zum Zitat Brody GH. Sibling relationship quality: Its causes and consequences. Ann Rev Psychol. 1998;49:1–24.CrossRef Brody GH. Sibling relationship quality: Its causes and consequences. Ann Rev Psychol. 1998;49:1–24.CrossRef
86.
Zurück zum Zitat Noller P. Sibling relationships in adolescence: learning and growing together. Pers Relatsh. 2005;12:1–22.CrossRef Noller P. Sibling relationships in adolescence: learning and growing together. Pers Relatsh. 2005;12:1–22.CrossRef
87.
Zurück zum Zitat Pare RD. Development in the family. Ann Rev Psychol. 2004;55:365–99.CrossRef Pare RD. Development in the family. Ann Rev Psychol. 2004;55:365–99.CrossRef
88.
Zurück zum Zitat Sanders R. Sibling relationships. Red Globe Press. 2004. Sanders R. Sibling relationships. Red Globe Press. 2004.
89.
Zurück zum Zitat Howe N, Ross H, Recchia H. Sibling relations in early childhood. In: Hart C, Smith PK, editors. New York. NY: Wiley; 2011. p. 356–72. Howe N, Ross H, Recchia H. Sibling relations in early childhood. In: Hart C, Smith PK, editors. New York. NY: Wiley; 2011. p. 356–72.
90.
Zurück zum Zitat Dunn J. Sibling relationships. In: Smith PK, Hart CH, editors. Blackwell handbook of childhood social development. Malden, Mass: Blackwell Publishing; 2002. p. 223–37. Dunn J. Sibling relationships. In: Smith PK, Hart CH, editors. Blackwell handbook of childhood social development. Malden, Mass: Blackwell Publishing; 2002. p. 223–37.
91.
Zurück zum Zitat Howe N, Recchia H. Sibling relations and their impact on children’s development. In: Tremblay RE, Barr RG, Peters RDeV, editors. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. Montreal, Quebec: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development; 2006. p. 1–8. Howe N, Recchia H. Sibling relations and their impact on children’s development. In: Tremblay RE, Barr RG, Peters RDeV, editors. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. Montreal, Quebec: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development; 2006. p. 1–8.
92.
Zurück zum Zitat Goldberg S, Grusec JE, Jenkins JM. Confidence in protection: arguments for a narrow definition of attachment. J Fam Psychol. 1999;13:475–83.CrossRef Goldberg S, Grusec JE, Jenkins JM. Confidence in protection: arguments for a narrow definition of attachment. J Fam Psychol. 1999;13:475–83.CrossRef
93.
Zurück zum Zitat Garner PW, Jones DC, Miner JL. Social competence among low-income preschoolers: emotion socialization practices and social cognitive correlates. Child Dev. 1994;65:622–37.PubMedCrossRef Garner PW, Jones DC, Miner JL. Social competence among low-income preschoolers: emotion socialization practices and social cognitive correlates. Child Dev. 1994;65:622–37.PubMedCrossRef
94.
Zurück zum Zitat White N, Ensor R, Marks A, Jacobs L, Hughes C. “It’s mine!” does sharing with siblings at age 3 predict sharing with siblings, friends, and unfamiliar peers at age 6? Early Educ Dev. 2014;25:185–201.CrossRef White N, Ensor R, Marks A, Jacobs L, Hughes C. “It’s mine!” does sharing with siblings at age 3 predict sharing with siblings, friends, and unfamiliar peers at age 6? Early Educ Dev. 2014;25:185–201.CrossRef
95.
Zurück zum Zitat Open Science Collaboration. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science. 2015;349:aac4716.CrossRef Open Science Collaboration. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science. 2015;349:aac4716.CrossRef
96.
Zurück zum Zitat Hinz D, Bauer M, Röder S, Olek S, Huehn J, Sack U, Borte M, Simon JC, Lehmann I, Herberth G, LINA study group. Cord blood tregs with stable FOXP3 expression are influenced by prenatal environment and associated with atopic dermatitis at the age of one year. Allergy. 2012;67:380–9.PubMedCrossRef Hinz D, Bauer M, Röder S, Olek S, Huehn J, Sack U, Borte M, Simon JC, Lehmann I, Herberth G, LINA study group. Cord blood tregs with stable FOXP3 expression are influenced by prenatal environment and associated with atopic dermatitis at the age of one year. Allergy. 2012;67:380–9.PubMedCrossRef
97.
Zurück zum Zitat Fliege H, Rose M, Arck P, Walter OB, Kocalevent RD, Weber C, Klapp BF. The perceived stress questionnaire. PSQ; reconsidered: validation and reference values from different clinical and healthy adult samples. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:78–88.PubMedCrossRef Fliege H, Rose M, Arck P, Walter OB, Kocalevent RD, Weber C, Klapp BF. The perceived stress questionnaire. PSQ; reconsidered: validation and reference values from different clinical and healthy adult samples. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:78–88.PubMedCrossRef
98.
Zurück zum Zitat Levenstein S, Prantera C, Varvo V, Scribano ML, Berto E, Luzi C, Andreoli A. Development of the perceived stress questionnaire: a new tool for psychosomatic research. J Psychosom Res. 1993;37:19–32.PubMedCrossRef Levenstein S, Prantera C, Varvo V, Scribano ML, Berto E, Luzi C, Andreoli A. Development of the perceived stress questionnaire: a new tool for psychosomatic research. J Psychosom Res. 1993;37:19–32.PubMedCrossRef
99.
Zurück zum Zitat Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:581–6.PubMedCrossRef Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:581–6.PubMedCrossRef
100.
Zurück zum Zitat Brühl B, Döpfner M, Lehmkuhl G. Der Fremdbeurteilungsbogen für hyperkinetische Störungen. FBB-HKS; - Prävalenz hyperkinetischer Störungen im Elternurteil und psychometrische Kriterien. Kindh Entwickl. 2000;9:116–26.CrossRef Brühl B, Döpfner M, Lehmkuhl G. Der Fremdbeurteilungsbogen für hyperkinetische Störungen. FBB-HKS; - Prävalenz hyperkinetischer Störungen im Elternurteil und psychometrische Kriterien. Kindh Entwickl. 2000;9:116–26.CrossRef
101.
Zurück zum Zitat Schneider SL, Kogan I. The international standard classification of education 1997: Challenges in the application to national data and the implementation in crossnational surveys. In: Schneider SL, editor. The international standard classification of education (ISCED-97). An evaluation of content and criterion validity for 15 European countries. Mannheim: MZES; 2008. p. 13–46. Schneider SL, Kogan I. The international standard classification of education 1997: Challenges in the application to national data and the implementation in crossnational surveys. In: Schneider SL, editor. The international standard classification of education (ISCED-97). An evaluation of content and criterion validity for 15 European countries. Mannheim: MZES; 2008. p. 13–46.
102.
Zurück zum Zitat Pickett KE, Wilkinson RG. Income inequality and health: a causal review. Soc Sci Med. 2015;128:316–26.PubMedCrossRef Pickett KE, Wilkinson RG. Income inequality and health: a causal review. Soc Sci Med. 2015;128:316–26.PubMedCrossRef
103.
Zurück zum Zitat Swartz JR, Knodt AR, Radtke SR, Hariri AR. Post-secondary maternal education buffers against neural risk for psychological vulnerability to future life stress. Neuropsychol. 2018;109:134–9.CrossRef Swartz JR, Knodt AR, Radtke SR, Hariri AR. Post-secondary maternal education buffers against neural risk for psychological vulnerability to future life stress. Neuropsychol. 2018;109:134–9.CrossRef
104.
Zurück zum Zitat Kahneman D, Krueger AB, Schkade DA, Schwarz N, Stone AA. A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: the day reconstruction method. Science. 2004;306:1776–80.PubMedCrossRef Kahneman D, Krueger AB, Schkade DA, Schwarz N, Stone AA. A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: the day reconstruction method. Science. 2004;306:1776–80.PubMedCrossRef
105.
Zurück zum Zitat Scher A, Sharabany R. Parenting anxiety and stress: does gender play a part at 3 months of age? J Genet Psychol. 2005;166:203–13.PubMedCrossRef Scher A, Sharabany R. Parenting anxiety and stress: does gender play a part at 3 months of age? J Genet Psychol. 2005;166:203–13.PubMedCrossRef
106.
Zurück zum Zitat Donald KA, Wedderburn CJ, Barnett W, Nhapi RT, Rehman AM, Stadler JAM, Hoffman N, Koen N, Zar HJ, Stein DJ. Risk and protective factors for child development: an observational South African birth cohort. PLoS Med. 2019;16:e1002920.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Donald KA, Wedderburn CJ, Barnett W, Nhapi RT, Rehman AM, Stadler JAM, Hoffman N, Koen N, Zar HJ, Stein DJ. Risk and protective factors for child development: an observational South African birth cohort. PLoS Med. 2019;16:e1002920.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
107.
Zurück zum Zitat Walker SP, Wachs TD, Grantham-McGregor S, Black MM, Nelson CA, Huffman SL, Baker-Henningham H, Chang SM, Hamadani JD, Lozoff B, Gardner JM, Powell CA, Rahman A, Richter L. Inequality in early childhood: risk and protective factors for early child development. Lancet. 2011;378:1325–38.PubMedCrossRef Walker SP, Wachs TD, Grantham-McGregor S, Black MM, Nelson CA, Huffman SL, Baker-Henningham H, Chang SM, Hamadani JD, Lozoff B, Gardner JM, Powell CA, Rahman A, Richter L. Inequality in early childhood: risk and protective factors for early child development. Lancet. 2011;378:1325–38.PubMedCrossRef
109.
Zurück zum Zitat Brooks M, Kristensen K, van Benthem KJ, Magnusson A, Berg CW, Nielsen A, Skaug HJ, Mächler M, Bolker BM. glmmTMB balances speed and flexibility among packages for zero-inflated Generalized linear mixed modeling. R J. 2017;9:378–400.CrossRef Brooks M, Kristensen K, van Benthem KJ, Magnusson A, Berg CW, Nielsen A, Skaug HJ, Mächler M, Bolker BM. glmmTMB balances speed and flexibility among packages for zero-inflated Generalized linear mixed modeling. R J. 2017;9:378–400.CrossRef
110.
Zurück zum Zitat Zimmerman FJ, Katon W. Socioeconomic status, depression disparities, and financial strain: what lies behind the income-depression relationship? Health Econ. 2005;14:1197–215.PubMedCrossRef Zimmerman FJ, Katon W. Socioeconomic status, depression disparities, and financial strain: what lies behind the income-depression relationship? Health Econ. 2005;14:1197–215.PubMedCrossRef
111.
Zurück zum Zitat Hasson R, Goldenring FJ. Gender differences among children with ADHD on continuous performance tests: a meta-analytic review. J Atten Disord. 2012;16:190–8.PubMedCrossRef Hasson R, Goldenring FJ. Gender differences among children with ADHD on continuous performance tests: a meta-analytic review. J Atten Disord. 2012;16:190–8.PubMedCrossRef
112.
Zurück zum Zitat Smithson M, Verkuilen J. A better lemon squeezer? Maximum-likelihood regression with beta-distributed dependent variables. Psychol Methods. 2006;11:54–71.PubMedCrossRef Smithson M, Verkuilen J. A better lemon squeezer? Maximum-likelihood regression with beta-distributed dependent variables. Psychol Methods. 2006;11:54–71.PubMedCrossRef
113.
Zurück zum Zitat Dobson A, Zidek J, Lindsey J. An introduction to generalized linear models. London: Chapman and Hall; 2001. Dobson A, Zidek J, Lindsey J. An introduction to generalized linear models. London: Chapman and Hall; 2001.
114.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosseel Y. Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling and moreVersion 0.5–12 BETA. J Stat Softw. 2012;48:1–36.CrossRef Rosseel Y. Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling and moreVersion 0.5–12 BETA. J Stat Softw. 2012;48:1–36.CrossRef
115.
Zurück zum Zitat Kievit RA, Brandmaier AM, Ziegler G, van Harmelen AL, de Mooij SMM, Moutoussis M, Goodyer IM, Bullmore E, Jones PB, Fonagy P, Consortium NSPN, Lindenberger U, Dolan RJ. Developmental cognitive neuroscience using latent change score models: a tutorial and applications. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018;33:99–117.PubMedCrossRef Kievit RA, Brandmaier AM, Ziegler G, van Harmelen AL, de Mooij SMM, Moutoussis M, Goodyer IM, Bullmore E, Jones PB, Fonagy P, Consortium NSPN, Lindenberger U, Dolan RJ. Developmental cognitive neuroscience using latent change score models: a tutorial and applications. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018;33:99–117.PubMedCrossRef
116.
Zurück zum Zitat Geiser C. Data analysis with MplusNew York. NY: Guilford Press; 2012. Geiser C. Data analysis with MplusNew York. NY: Guilford Press; 2012.
117.
Zurück zum Zitat Woerner W, Becker A, Rothenberger A. Normative data and scale properties of the German parent SDQ. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004;13:3–10. Woerner W, Becker A, Rothenberger A. Normative data and scale properties of the German parent SDQ. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004;13:3–10.
118.
Zurück zum Zitat Woerner W, Becker A, Friedrich C, Klasen H, Goodman R, Rothenberger A. Normierung und Evaluation der deutschen Elternversion des Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): Ergebnisse einer repräsentativen Felderhebung. Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr. 2002;30:105–12.CrossRef Woerner W, Becker A, Friedrich C, Klasen H, Goodman R, Rothenberger A. Normierung und Evaluation der deutschen Elternversion des Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): Ergebnisse einer repräsentativen Felderhebung. Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr. 2002;30:105–12.CrossRef
119.
120.
Zurück zum Zitat Mitchell R, Popham F. Effect of exposure to natural environment on health inequalities: an observational population study. Lancet. 2008;372:1655–60.PubMedCrossRef Mitchell R, Popham F. Effect of exposure to natural environment on health inequalities: an observational population study. Lancet. 2008;372:1655–60.PubMedCrossRef
121.
Zurück zum Zitat Leipzig S. Lebendig grüne Stadt am Wasser. Leipzig: Freiraumstrategie der Stadt Leipzig; 2017. Leipzig S. Lebendig grüne Stadt am Wasser. Leipzig: Freiraumstrategie der Stadt Leipzig; 2017.
122.
Zurück zum Zitat Chan JC, Nugent BM, Bale TL. Parental advisory: maternal and paternal stress can impact offspring neurodevelopment. Biol Psychiatry. 2017;83:886–94.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Chan JC, Nugent BM, Bale TL. Parental advisory: maternal and paternal stress can impact offspring neurodevelopment. Biol Psychiatry. 2017;83:886–94.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
123.
Zurück zum Zitat Dunn J. Siblings and socialization. In: Grusec JE, Hastings PD, editors. Handbook of socialization: theory and research. New York: Guilford Press; 2001. p. 309–27. Dunn J. Siblings and socialization. In: Grusec JE, Hastings PD, editors. Handbook of socialization: theory and research. New York: Guilford Press; 2001. p. 309–27.
124.
Zurück zum Zitat Howe N, Rinaldi CM, Jennings M, Petrakos H. No! The lambs can stay out because they got cozies! Child Dev. 2002;73:1460–73.PubMedCrossRef Howe N, Rinaldi CM, Jennings M, Petrakos H. No! The lambs can stay out because they got cozies! Child Dev. 2002;73:1460–73.PubMedCrossRef
125.
Zurück zum Zitat Shepherd D, Goedeke S, Landon J, Taylor S, Williams J. The impact of sibling relationships on later-life psychological and subjective well-being. J Adult Dev. 2020;28:76–86.CrossRef Shepherd D, Goedeke S, Landon J, Taylor S, Williams J. The impact of sibling relationships on later-life psychological and subjective well-being. J Adult Dev. 2020;28:76–86.CrossRef
126.
Zurück zum Zitat Youngblade LM, Dunn J. Individual differences in young children’s pretend play with mother and sibling: links to relationships and understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs. Child Dev. 1995;66:1472–92.PubMedCrossRef Youngblade LM, Dunn J. Individual differences in young children’s pretend play with mother and sibling: links to relationships and understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs. Child Dev. 1995;66:1472–92.PubMedCrossRef
127.
Zurück zum Zitat Whiteman SD, Buchanan CM. Mothers’ and children’s expectations for adolescence: the impact of perceptions of an older sibling’s experience. J Fam Psychol. 2002;16:157–71.PubMedCrossRef Whiteman SD, Buchanan CM. Mothers’ and children’s expectations for adolescence: the impact of perceptions of an older sibling’s experience. J Fam Psychol. 2002;16:157–71.PubMedCrossRef
128.
Zurück zum Zitat Whiteman SD, McHale SM, Crouter AC. What parents learn from experience: the first child as a first draft? J Marriage Fam. 2003;65:608–21.CrossRef Whiteman SD, McHale SM, Crouter AC. What parents learn from experience: the first child as a first draft? J Marriage Fam. 2003;65:608–21.CrossRef
129.
Zurück zum Zitat Marzocchi GM, Capron C, Pietro MD, Tauleria ED, Duyme M, Frigerio A, Gaspar MF, Hamilton H, Pithon G, Simões A, Thérond C. The use of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. SDQ in Southern European countries. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004;13:40–6.CrossRef Marzocchi GM, Capron C, Pietro MD, Tauleria ED, Duyme M, Frigerio A, Gaspar MF, Hamilton H, Pithon G, Simões A, Thérond C. The use of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. SDQ in Southern European countries. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004;13:40–6.CrossRef
130.
Zurück zum Zitat Updegraff KA, McHale SM, Killoren SE, Rodriguez SA. Cultural variations in sibling relationships. In: Caspi J, editor. Sibling development: implications for mental health practitioners. New York: Springer; 2010. p. 83–105. Updegraff KA, McHale SM, Killoren SE, Rodriguez SA. Cultural variations in sibling relationships. In: Caspi J, editor. Sibling development: implications for mental health practitioners. New York: Springer; 2010. p. 83–105.
Metadaten
Titel
Maternal stress, child behavior and the promotive role of older siblings
verfasst von
Federica Amici
Stefan Röder
Wieland Kiess
Michael Borte
Ana C. Zenclussen
Anja Widdig
Gunda Herberth
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2022
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Public Health / Ausgabe 1/2022
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2458
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-022-13261-2

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2022

BMC Public Health 1/2022 Zur Ausgabe