Skip to main content
Erschienen in: The Egyptian Journal of Neurology, Psychiatry and Neurosurgery 1/2022

Open Access 01.12.2022 | Research

Training second-grade dyslexic students using a computerized program in Asyut, Egypt

verfasst von: W. M. Farghaly, S. H. El Tallawy, A. S. Ramadan, R. B. Abdelrasol, M. S. Omar, H. N. Eltallawy, K. O. Mohamed

Erschienen in: The Egyptian Journal of Neurology, Psychiatry and Neurosurgery | Ausgabe 1/2022

Abstract

Background

Dyslexia is characterized by difficulties in spelling and reading. The aim of this study is to identify domains of cognitive strength and weakness of students with dyslexia and to study the effect of an especially designed computerized training program on their reading abilities. This study was conducted on 2nd-grade primary school students in Asyut city, Egypt (n = 560). Identification of students with dyslexia was done using Arabic Reading Achievement test and a newly constructed computerized Arabic Cognitive Abilities diagnostic battery for Reading (CADB-R). Training was applied using a newly constructed Computerized Cognitive abilities training battery for reading (CATB-R).

Results

The prevalence of dyslexia was 13.9% (N = 52/373), and it was higher among girls (15.8%) than boys (11.7%). After application of the training program, there was a significant increase in post-training mean scores of CADB-R score in their total and all sub-items.

Conclusions

All struggling readers should be included in well-tailored research-based rehabilitation programs.
Clinical trial registration Training Second-grade Dyslexic Students Using a Computerized Program in Assiut, Egypt (Dyslexia), NCT04642859, 12 November 2020. URL: https://​register.​clinicaltrials.​gov/​prs/​app/​action/​Status?​uid=​U000495F&​ts=​10&​sid=​S000AD3G&​cx=​20acrp
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s41983-022-00480-y.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
ARAT
Arabic reading achievement test
CADB-R
Cognitive abilities diagnostic battery for reading
CATB-R
Cognitive abilities training battery for reading

Background

Dyslexia is a specific learning disability that is neurological in origin. It is characterized by difficulties with accurate or fluent word recognition, by poor spelling and decoding abilities, difficulty reading words in isolation, and difficulty with oral reading (slow, inaccurate, or labored). These difficulties typically result from a deficit in the phonological component of language that is often unexpected with other cognitive abilities and the provision of effective classroom instruction [1].
The phonological deficit hypothesis is the dominant explanatory theory of developmental dyslexia [2, 3]. Despite this, impaired phonological processing alone cannot explain all clinical symptoms of dyslexia. Many students with dyslexia have multiple deficits other than phonological deficits [48]. Of these accused deficits are visual perceptual processing deficits [9], auditory processing deficits [10], multisensory spatial attention deficits [11], as well as cerebellar dysfunction [12].
The current mandatory practical criterion for the diagnosis of dyslexia, below-average achievement, implies waiting to failure. This approach for diagnosis (must fail approach) would delay intervention for rehabilitation. Furthermore, the cut-off scores would result in over or under inclusion of cases and appear to be the least reliable and valid approach to diagnosis. Both methods lack a deeper understanding of the underlying reading difficulty, their neurobiological basis, and hence represent barriers against scientifically based interventions. However, improved understanding of the neurobiological basis of dyslexia will facilitate evidence-based effective intervention.
The aim of this work is to identify the domains of cognitive strength and weakness of students struggling with reading and to study the effect of an especially designed computerized training program on their reading abilities.

Methods

Study group

This intervention study was conducted on 2nd-grade primary school students chosen from 6 geographically distributed governmental schools in Asyut city, Egypt. One classroom was randomly selected from each school (n = 560). Students with dyslexia were chosen out of this sample. They fulfilled the following criteria:
(1)
Poor scholastic achievement: Obtaining less than mean–standard deviation (SD) score on a standardized Arabic reading achievement test specifically designed for this study;
 
(2)
Normal neurological, basic audiological, and ophthalmological examination.
 

Control group

The control group was selected from 2nd-grade primary school students of the same public schools who fulfilled the following criteria:
(1)
Good scholastic achievement (Obtaining score more than mean + SD on a standardized Arabic achievement test);
 
(2)
Normal neurological, basic audiological, and ophthalmological examination.
 

Methods

Ethical consideration: Informed written consent was signed by the parent or legal guardian of the student. This study protocol was approved by the ethical committee of Asyut university faculty of medicine with ethical approval number: 2015-03-R20 and 2015-3-R21. Also this study protocol was registered in Clinical trial.gov with Identifier: NCT04642859.
The current study was carried out along seven stages:

First stage: identification of students with reading disability (Dyslexia)

a.
Construction of Arabic Reading Achievement Test: it was carried out by 2 professors of educational psychology (see Additional file 1). Then its validity and reliability were assessed in a pilot study on 185 2nd-grade students, and were correlated with their school scores.
 
b.
All students in 2nd grade from the chosen six public primary schools in Asyut city (n = 560 students) were included in this study except those who were absent during their school visits (n = 84) or those who refused to participate in this study (n = 103). The rest of the sample (n = 373) completed this study under the supervision of 4 neuropsychiatrists, two expert psychometrists, and 12 social workers.
 
All students (n = 373 students) were subjected to the following:
(1)
Evaluation of achievement level: by the new scale of Arabic Reading Achievement Test (ARAT), specially designed for this study. Students were classified according to their achievement level into three groups:
a)
Group I: Poor achievers (scores < mean − SD), considered students with reading disability or dyslexia (our target in this study) (n = 52 students)
 
b)
Group II: Average achievers (with scores = mean ± SD) (n = 295 students)
 
c)
Group III included those with good achievement (score > mean + SD) and were considered as a control group (n = 26 students).
Thereafter, the students with reading disability were subjected to the following:
 
 
(2)
Audiological assessments: audiometry and tympanometry to exclude peripheral hearing loss.
 
(3)
Ophthalmological examination: visual acuity by counting fingers at 6 m to exclude gross visual deficits.
 

Second stage: construction of a computerized Arabic cognitive abilities diagnostic battery for reading (CADB-R)

This stage was carried out by seven expert staff members in the fields of neurology and education. The constructed battery was designed in a game‑like manner to test a wide range of cognitive skills that are supposed to be important in the early stages of learning to read. It includes the following skills:
(1)
Text Reading comprehension: It includes give word meaning, true or false, give the opposite, plural, singular, match the word with an appropriate picture, and match the sentence with the appropriate picture.
 
(2)
Visual discrimination: it includes letter identification and word identification.
 
(3)
Auditory discrimination: Al-tanween, Al-mad, and Al-shad.
 
(4)
Phonological awareness: It includes: match pictures of rhyming words, give words of the same rhyme, blending of syllables, heard words rhyme or not, match pictures starting with the same letter.
 
(5)
Visuospatial skills: it includes identification of letter relations to others, proper dotting of letters, and word formation from letters.
 
(6)
Audio-Visual correspondence: identification of nonverbal sounds, match the heard word with the proper picture, word segmentation, substitute the 1st letter and pronounce the word, identification of words starting with the heard letter sound, and complete with appropriate syllable.
 
(7)
Auditory memory: it includes the memory of sentences and memory of words.
 

Third stage: standardization of the constructed diagnostic battery

(1)
Reliability: by assessment of internal consistency reliability, which in turn depends on the measurement of values of Cronbach’s Alpha correlation coefficient [13], and corrected item–total correlation coefficient [14].
 
(2)
Validity of the constructed battery depends on measuring the following:
a)
Judgment validity: the test was judged by well‑experienced referees (7 experts; 4 of neuropsychiatry medical field; and 3 of educational field) to show the relevance and appropriateness of individual test items to the study purpose
 
b)
Contrasted group validity: t‑test was used for comparison of scores of the reading cognitive abilities of students with dyslexia (group I) and the control group (group III), using the newly constructed Cognitive Abilities Diagnostic Battery for Reading (CADB-R)
 
c)
Diagnostic validity: sensitivity, specificity, positive predictive value, and negative predictive value were used to determine diagnostic validity.
 
 

Fourth stage: detailed study of the cognitive profile

Study the cognitive profiles of students with dyslexia (n = 52) using the newly constructed CADB-R to identify their cognitive skill deficit or deficits that might contribute to their disability (dyslexia).

Fifth stage: construction of a computerized cognitive abilities training battery for reading (CATB-R)

This constructed training battery will be used for the development and enhancement of cognitive skills that contribute to learning to read. The constructed training battery is composed of the same visual and auditory cognitive skills of the diagnostic battery. Still, it is a more extended game like (multiple levels of increasing difficulty) and has unlimited time. The child is not allowed to pass to the next level until he masters the current one.

Sixth stage: application of the training battery (CATB-R)

Only 16 students with dyslexia accepted to continue the next stage of the study (rehabilitation and reassessment). Rehabilitation was carried out in the neuroepidemiology centre at Asyut University, along two months. Each student attended with one or both his/her parents received five sessions per week. Each session lasted for 90–120 min with intervening one or two brakes in between. Two sessions were held per day (from 9 to 11 and from 12 to 14), and each student’s parents were allowed to choose their suitable sessions for attendance.

Seventh stage: reassessment

Reassessment of students with dyslexia at the end of the training program was done, using the CADB-R for evaluation of their cognitive skill abilities after rehabilitation. Their reading achievement level was also reassessed after training using Arabic Reading Achievement test (ARAT) (Additional file 1).

Results

Arabic reading achievement test (ARAT)

Reliability and validity of the newly constructed Arabic Reading Achievement Test (ARAT) were assessed in a pilot study on 185 2nd-grade primary school students. Cronbach’s alpha coefficient value of ARAT was 0.785. It showed minimal decrease following the deletion of any component of the achievement test, and this means high internal consistency of ARAT. Pearson’s correlation revealed a significant correlation between the newly constructed ARAT and end-year Arabic achievement school test (r = 0.601) (Criterion-related validity). Using the newly constructed ARAT, it was found that the prevalence of dyslexia among 2nd-grade public school students of Asyut city was 13.9% (N = 52/373), and it was higher among girls (15.8%) than boys (11.7%) (Table 1).
Table 1
Reading achievement level of 2nd-grade public school students
Arabic achievement level
Arabic (N = 373)
Male
Female
Total
N
%
N
%
N
%
Group I: Poor achievers
20
11.7
32
15.8
52
13.9
Group II: Average achievers
137
80.1
158
78.2
295
79.1
Group III: Good achievers
14
8.2
12
5.9
26
6.97
Total
171
45.8
202
54.2
373
100
Table 1 shows Reading achievement level of 2nd-grade public school students. The prevalence of dyslexia among 2nd-grade public school students of Asyut city was 13.9% (N = 52/373), and it was higher among girls (15.8%) than boys (11.7%).

Standardization of cognitive abilities diagnostic battery for reading (CADB-R)

The newly constructed CADB-R for students with dyslexia has high internal consistency with Cronbach’s alpha coefficient value of 0.741. Comparison between mean scores of cognitive skill abilities of poor and good achievers among 2nd-grade public school students of Asyut city shows that students with poor Arabic academic achievement have significantly lower mean score on the total and all sub-items of the CADB-R than their good achieving peers (contrasted group validity) (Table 2).
Table 2
Contrasted group validity
Studied reading cognitive skill
Poor achiever (N = 52)
Good achiever (N = 26)
T
P-value
95% CI
Mean ± SD
Mean ± SD
Reading achievement test
18.46 ± 5.65
40.0. ± 0.0
19.37
0.0001
− 23.75: − 19.32
Reading comprehension
55.56 ± 8.39
62.96 ± 6.22
9.89
0.0001
− 22.11: − 14.47
Visual discrimination
21.17 ± 3.60
23.77 ± 0.65
3.64
0.0001
− 4.02: − 1.18
Auditory discrimination
9.38 ± 4.28
17.92 ± 3.32
8.90
0.0001
− 10.45: − 6.63
Phonological awareness
14.63 ± 5.03
24.19 ± 4.24
8.65
0.0001
− 11.09: − 6.95
Visuospatial skills
12.21 ± 4.05
18.08 ± 2.21
6.88
0.0001
− 7.56: − 4.17
Audio-visual correspondence
21.27 ± 4.18
28.77 ± 2.07
8.61
0.0001
9.23: − 5.77
Auditory memory
5.87 ± 2.21
8.12 ± 1.40
4.73
0.002
− 3.20: − 1.30
Reading ability score
129.10 ± 23.97
184.19 ± 13.74
10.84
0.0001
− 65.22: − 44.97
The CADB-R for students with dyslexia had sensitivity 86.5%, specificity 84.4%, positive predictive value 47.4%, and negative predictive value 97.5% (Table 3).
Table 3
Diagnostic validity of CADB-R
Studied cognitive skill
TP
TN
FP
FN
Sensitivity
Specificity
PPV
NPV
Reading comprehension
44
264
57
8
84.6%
82%
43.5%
97%
Auditory-Visual discrimination
41
271
50
11
79%
84%
45%
96%
Auditory discrimination
42
278
43
10
80.8%
87%
49%
97%
Phonological awareness
39
261
60
13
75%
81%
41%
95%
Visuospatial skills
39
266
55
13
75%
83%
41%
95%
Visual discrimination
28
295
26
24
0.0
97
0.0
0.0
Auditory memory
30
260
61
22
58%
81%
33%
92.2%
Total cognitive skills for reading
45
271
50
7
86.5%
84%
45.9%
97%
*TP = True Positive (dyslexics scoring < cut-off); TN = True Negative (control scoring > cut-off), FP = False Positive (control scoring < cut-off); FN = False Negative (dyslexics scoring > cut-off); Sensitivity = TP/(TP + FN); Specificity = TN/(TN + FP); Positive Predictive Value (PPV) = TP/(TP + FP); Negative Predictive Value (NPV) = TN/(TN + FN)
Multiple correlations between Arabic achievement test as a dependent variable and the studied cognitive skills of the battery as independent variables revealed that 61% of variations in Arabic achievement scores are attributed to these studied cognitive skills collectively, and the remaining 39% are attributed to other factors (Table 4). Multiple correlations between Arabic achievement test as a dependent variable and sub-items of the cognitive skill battery, as independent variables, revealed that audio-visual discrimination had the highest contribution (47%) upon which Arabic achievement level depends (Table 4).
Table 4
Multiple correlation coefficient test
Dyslexia battery
Correlation Coefficient
Relative efficiency
Coefficient of determination
Coefficient of multiple correlation
F-Ratio (regression)
Arabic achievement test
P.R
R2
Efficiency
Each item
Total
Each item
Total
Each item
Total
Reading comprehension
0.69
0.18
0.59
9.18
39.50%
61%
62.80%
78%
29.66
94.02
Audio-Visual correspondence
0.67
0.30
0.57
27.43
47%
69%
40.72
Auditory Discrimination
0.65
0.21
0.59
12.82
46%
68%
104.33
Phonological awareness
0.53
0.09
0.6
2.36
37%
61%
43.29
Visuospatial skills
0.59
0.16
0.6
7.11
39%
63%
79.87
Auditory memory
0.39
0.08
0.6
1.75
22%
46%
50.84
Number of Cases (373); PROB > (0.001)
Table 2 shows contrasted group validity. Comparison between mean scores of cognitive skill abilities of poor and good achievers among 2nd-grade public school students of Asyut city shows that students with poor Arabic academic achievement have significantly lower mean score on the total and all sub-items of the CADB-R than their good achieving peers.
Table 3 shows Diagnostic Validity of CADB-R. The CADB-R for students with dyslexia had sensitivity 86.5%, specificity 84.4%, positive predictive value 47.4%, and negative predictive value 97.5%
Table 4 shows multiple correlation coefficient test. Multiple correlations between Arabic achievement test as a dependent variable and the studied cognitive skills of the battery as independent variables revealed that 61% of variations in Arabic achievement scores are attributed to these studied cognitive skills collectively, and the remaining 39% are attributed to other factors. Multiple correlations between Arabic achievement test as a dependent variable and sub-items of the cognitive skill battery, as independent variables, revealed that audio-visual discrimination had the highest contribution (47%) upon which Arabic achievement level depends.

Pre- and post-training mean scores of cognitive skill abilities

There was a significant increase in post-training mean scores of the total (P-value 0.000) as well as sub-items of the tested cognitive skill abilities of students with dyslexia (N = 16) (Fig. 1). Reassessment of Arabic achievement level of the trainee sample (N = 16 students) revealed significant improvement in the total and all sub-items of Arabic achievement test (Fig. 2).

Discussion

Dyslexia occurs across all languages (alphabetic languages, such as English, French, German, Arabic, and logographic languages, such as Chinese and Hieroglyphs). It is recorded in all socioeconomic statuses, cultures, and ethnic backgrounds [15]. However, the prevalence of dyslexia is not the same in all countries. In Finnish, there is a good correlation between orthography (the way a letter looks) and phonology (the way it sounds). It is easy for children to read, and so there is a lower prevalence of reading disability in countries speaking Finnish [16]. On the other hand, in English, the relationship between orthography and phonology is inconsistent. One letter can represent several different sounds. For example, the letter “s” is pronounced differently in each of the words “sun”, “sugar”, and “lens”, and the sound of the letter “F” is written differently in “left” and “elephant”. Thus, the prevalence of dyslexia is higher among English speakers than among speakers of many other languages [16]. In the USA, reading disabilities (dyslexia) comprise 80% of all learning disabilities, and as many as 43.5 million Americans have dyslexia [15]. Again Arabic is a language with deep orthography, for example, the letter “Alef” (أ is pronounced differently in different words as أ أسامه, أحمد, إسلام and hence has a high prevalence of dyslexia that was also found by Naama Friedmann, Manar Haddad-Hanna (2012) [17].
Using the specifically constructed Arabic Reading Achievement Test (ARAT), it was found that the prevalence of dyslexia among 2nd-grade public school students of Asyut city was 13.9%, N = 52/373. It lies at the upper range of the worldwide, estimates of prevalence which vary from 2 to 15% of the population. It has acceptable reliability according to National Institutes of Health 2007, which reported that, for the tests to be reliable, its Cronbach’s value should be 0.70 or more. Moreover, deletion of any of its constituent sub-items shows a minimal decrease in its Cronbach’s alpha value, which indicates the significance of all constituent sub-items [14, 18].
Despite the proved validity and reliability of the constructed ARAT (Cronbach’s alpha 0.785 & Pearson’ correlation r = 0.601 with end-year Arabic achievement school test) in the identification of students with dyslexia, it is better for all students who are struggling in the early stages of learning to read, to be subjected to detailed study and rehabilitation programs, not to wait to proven failure.
The specifically constructed Cognitive Ability Diagnostic Battery for Reading (CADB-R) had Cronbach’s alpha coefficient value 0.741 and all values of its corrected item–total correlation were 0.2 or more, which indicates acceptable reliability [13, 14]. Again, deletion of any sub-item of the diagnostic battery results in a minimal decrease of the total Cronbach’s alpha value indicating internal consistency and high reliability of the total battery and its sub-items.
Regarding the studied reading cognitive skills, the students with poor Arabic academic achievement had significantly lower scores than those with good Arabic academic achievement at the total and all sub-items of the CADB-R (Fig. 1).
The significantly low scores of poor achievers compared with good achievers on the text reading comprehension task of the battery (P < 0.001) may indicate the importance of comprehension as a cognitive process intimately related to reading and justifies its deficit as underlying pathophysiology of dyslexia. This impairment of comprehension tasks might be attributed to poor vocabulary store of those children. This may highlight the necessity of early enrichment of vocabulary store of young children to improve their comprehension strategies. This finding is supported by the significantly impaired mean scores of giving the word meaning or opposite word sub-test obtained by children with dyslexia compared to good readers. Impaired comprehension results in low responding to teacher’s instructions, in re-reading of the text more and more with consequent slow reading and low level of achievement. Consistent with these results are the findings of Vellutino in 1991, who mentioned that failure to use good comprehension strategies could contribute to poor reading and reduced scores in spelling [19].
Over the past three decades, accumulated evidence demonstrates that dyslexia involves difficulty within the language system, specifically phonology [20]. The phonological deficit hypothesis relies on the converging evidence from laboratories around the world which demonstrated what has been termed “neural signature of dyslexia”. This describes the inefficient functioning of the left posterior reading system (Wernicke’s area, angular gyrus, and striate cortex) [21]. In the present study, poor achievers performed significantly worse than good achievers on phonological awareness (0.001). Difficulties in phonemic awareness are typically observed in students with dyslexia and impact a student’s ability to learn letters and the sounds associated with a given letter shape and to understand the alphabetic principle (audio-visual correspondence), resulting in difficulties in decoding words and accurate spelling. These findings are consistent with the study done by Gentile (2005), who considered a weakness in rhyme detection and phonological awareness as a precursor of dyslexia [22].
Students with dyslexia had lower visuospatial skills than good achievers (0.001). The significantly poor visuospatial skills of students with Arabic reading disability in comparison to good readers could be explained by their inability to discriminate between similar Arabic letters specially dotted letters as (ج خ ح), (ب ن). Moreover, these impaired visuospatial skills are the underlying contributor for letter reversal within a word. This view is confirmed by the significantly lower scores of poor readers on their ability to form words from letters, and their lower but insignificant lower scores on proper dotting of letters.
The significantly low scores of auditory memory tasks of the battery among poor achievers compared to good achievers (0.002) might indicate the importance of auditory memory deficits in the aetiology of dyslexia and justifies its importance in the newly constructed diagnostic battery. These results agree with the study of Slaghuis and Ryan, who found that dyslexic children have auditory memory deficit [23]. Poor auditory memory makes dyslexic students forget parts of words, phrases, or sentences before they have been completely understood, resulting in poor comprehension and this necessitates re-reading more and more with consequently slow reading [24]. Moreover, memory for letter patterns, letter sequences, and the letters in whole words (orthographic processing) may be selectively impaired or may coexist with phonological processing weaknesses and poor auditory memory that contribute to dyslexia [24].
In the present study, poor readers scored significantly lower scores on audio-visual correspondence tasks than their peers with good reading. The beginning reader should connect the shape of letters (graphemes) to the sounds of spoken letters, or the elemental particles of speech (phonemes), i.e., grapheme-phoneme correspondence. The phonological deficit hypothesis represents the most robust and specific correlate of reading disability [25]. And also, grapheme-phoneme correspondence or mastery of audio-visual correspondence skills are crucial not only for the beginning readers but also for early developing infant to connect the heard words to the corresponding objects around him. Thus, understanding this underlying pathophysiology represents the basis of most successful programs of an evidence-based intervention designed to improve reading.
Evaluation of sensitivity and specificity of the newly constructed Cognitive Abilities Diagnostic Battery for reading revealed that the new battery had high sensitivity as a screening tool (86.5%), and it is a useful tool for diagnosis of dyslexia (specificity = 84%) according to Cicchetti [26]. Moreover, some of its sub-items, e.g. visual discrimination (97%), auditory discrimination (87%), and audio-visual correspondence (84%) have excellent specificity for the diagnosis of dyslexia cases.
After training of sixteen students with reading disability was done, there was a significant improvement on the total battery score of cognitive skills as well as its sub-items. This improvement in reading cognitive abilities was associated with a significant improvement in reading achievement as evaluated by ARAT. These findings document the importance of the studied cognitive skills and their relevance to the development of the reading process.

Conclusions

Dyslexia is a disorder amenable for rehabilitation and improvement, particularly with early intervention during periods of fruitful neuroplasticity. Well-tailored research-based rehabilitation programs, according to identified points of weakness, could result in improvement of cognitive skills and, consequently, reading achievement.
Lastly, it is recommended to change the terminology to students with reading difficulties instead of reading disabilities, as it is amenable for rehabilitation and improvement. All struggling readers should be included in special education programs and offered services to meet their unique needs.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Authors’ information

WF, RA, GS, and HE are professors of neurology, Assiut university. MA and KO are associate professors of neurology, Assiut University. SE is an associate professor of Psychology—Faculty of Arts—Assiut University. AR is an associate professor of Hearing disability—Faculty of Sciences of Special Needs—Beni-Sueif University. MO is a professor of Educational Psychology—Faculty of Education—Assiut University.

Declarations

Informed written consent was signed by the parent or legal guardian of the student. This study protocol was approved by the ethical committee of Assiut university faculty of medicine with ethical approval number: Prof. Waffa Farghaly (2015-03-R20) and Prof. Reda Badry (2015-3-R21). Also this study protocol was registered in Clinical trial.gov with Identifier: NCT04642859.
Not applicable as the manuscript does not contain any individual person’s data in any form (individual details, images or videos).

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Adlof SM, Hogan TP. Understanding dyslexia in the context of developmental language disorders. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2018;49(4):762–73.CrossRef Adlof SM, Hogan TP. Understanding dyslexia in the context of developmental language disorders. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2018;49(4):762–73.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Bradley L, Bryant PE. Difficulties in auditory organisation as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature. 1978;271(5647):746–7.CrossRef Bradley L, Bryant PE. Difficulties in auditory organisation as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature. 1978;271(5647):746–7.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Stanovich KE. Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: the phonological-core variable-difference model. J Learn Disabil. 1988;21(10):590–604.CrossRef Stanovich KE. Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: the phonological-core variable-difference model. J Learn Disabil. 1988;21(10):590–604.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Georgiou GK, Papadopoulos TC, Zarouna E, Parrila R. Are auditory and visual processing deficits related to developmental dyslexia? Dyslexia. 2012;18(2):110–29.CrossRef Georgiou GK, Papadopoulos TC, Zarouna E, Parrila R. Are auditory and visual processing deficits related to developmental dyslexia? Dyslexia. 2012;18(2):110–29.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Pennington BF, Santerre-Lemmon L, Rosenberg J, MacDonald B, Boada R, Friend A, et al. Individual prediction of dyslexia by single versus multiple deficit models. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012;121(1):212–24.CrossRef Pennington BF, Santerre-Lemmon L, Rosenberg J, MacDonald B, Boada R, Friend A, et al. Individual prediction of dyslexia by single versus multiple deficit models. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012;121(1):212–24.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Peyrin C, Lallier M, Demonet JF, Pernet C, Baciu M, Le Bas JF, et al. Neural dissociation of phonological and visual attention span disorders in developmental dyslexia: FMRI evidence from two case reports. Brain Lang. 2012;120(3):381–94.CrossRef Peyrin C, Lallier M, Demonet JF, Pernet C, Baciu M, Le Bas JF, et al. Neural dissociation of phonological and visual attention span disorders in developmental dyslexia: FMRI evidence from two case reports. Brain Lang. 2012;120(3):381–94.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Ramus F, Rosen S, Dakin SC, Day BL, Castellote JM, White S, et al. Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain. 2003;126(Pt 4):841–65.CrossRef Ramus F, Rosen S, Dakin SC, Day BL, Castellote JM, White S, et al. Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain. 2003;126(Pt 4):841–65.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Valdois S, Bidet-Ildei C, Lassus-Sangosse D, Reilhac C, N’guyen-Morel M-A, Guinet E, et al. A visual processing but no phonological disorder in a child with mixed dyslexia. Cortex. 2011;47(10):1197–218.CrossRef Valdois S, Bidet-Ildei C, Lassus-Sangosse D, Reilhac C, N’guyen-Morel M-A, Guinet E, et al. A visual processing but no phonological disorder in a child with mixed dyslexia. Cortex. 2011;47(10):1197–218.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Stein J. The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia. 2001;7(1):12–36.CrossRef Stein J. The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia. 2001;7(1):12–36.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Facoetti A, Trussardi AN, Ruffino M, Lorusso ML, Cattaneo C, Galli R, et al. Multisensory spatial attention deficits are predictive of phonological decoding skills in developmental dyslexia. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22(5):1011–25.CrossRef Facoetti A, Trussardi AN, Ruffino M, Lorusso ML, Cattaneo C, Galli R, et al. Multisensory spatial attention deficits are predictive of phonological decoding skills in developmental dyslexia. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22(5):1011–25.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Stoodley CJ, Stein JF. The cerebellum and dyslexia. Cortex. 2011;47(1):101–16.CrossRef Stoodley CJ, Stein JF. The cerebellum and dyslexia. Cortex. 2011;47(1):101–16.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Olvera Astivia OL, Kroc E, Zumbo BD. The role of item distributions on reliability estimation: the case of Cronbach’s coefficient alpha. Educ Psychol Meas. 2020;80(5):825–46.CrossRef Olvera Astivia OL, Kroc E, Zumbo BD. The role of item distributions on reliability estimation: the case of Cronbach’s coefficient alpha. Educ Psychol Meas. 2020;80(5):825–46.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Revelle W, Condon DM. Reliability from α to ω: a tutorial. Psychol Assess. 2019;31(12):1395–411.CrossRef Revelle W, Condon DM. Reliability from α to ω: a tutorial. Psychol Assess. 2019;31(12):1395–411.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Peterson RL, Pennington BF. Developmental dyslexia. Ann Rev Clin Psychol. 2015;11:283–307.CrossRef Peterson RL, Pennington BF. Developmental dyslexia. Ann Rev Clin Psychol. 2015;11:283–307.CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Paulesu E, Demonet JF, Fazio F, McCrory E, Chanoine V, Brunswick N, et al. Dyslexia: cultural diversity and biological unity. Science. 2001;291(5511):2165–7.CrossRef Paulesu E, Demonet JF, Fazio F, McCrory E, Chanoine V, Brunswick N, et al. Dyslexia: cultural diversity and biological unity. Science. 2001;291(5511):2165–7.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Friedmann N, Haddad-Hanna M. Letter position dyslexia in Arabic: from form to position. Behav Neurol. 2012;25(3):193–203.CrossRef Friedmann N, Haddad-Hanna M. Letter position dyslexia in Arabic: from form to position. Behav Neurol. 2012;25(3):193–203.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Farghaly WM, Ahmed MA, Elmestikawy TA, Abdel-Rasool RB, Farghaly MS, Omar MS, et al. Construction of an Arabic computerized cognitive skill battery for the diagnosis of children with specific learning disabilities. J Curr Med Res Pract. 2016;1(3):38.CrossRef Farghaly WM, Ahmed MA, Elmestikawy TA, Abdel-Rasool RB, Farghaly MS, Omar MS, et al. Construction of an Arabic computerized cognitive skill battery for the diagnosis of children with specific learning disabilities. J Curr Med Res Pract. 2016;1(3):38.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Vellutino FR. Introduction to three studies on reading acquisition: convergent findings on theoretical foundations of code-oriented versus whole-language approaches to reading instruction. J Educ Psychol. 1991;83(4):437.CrossRef Vellutino FR. Introduction to three studies on reading acquisition: convergent findings on theoretical foundations of code-oriented versus whole-language approaches to reading instruction. J Educ Psychol. 1991;83(4):437.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Szenkovits G, Ramus F. Exploring dyslexics’ phonological deficit I: lexical vs sub-lexical and input vs output processes. Dyslexia. 2005;11(4):253–68.CrossRef Szenkovits G, Ramus F. Exploring dyslexics’ phonological deficit I: lexical vs sub-lexical and input vs output processes. Dyslexia. 2005;11(4):253–68.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA, Pugh KR, Fulbright RK, Constable RT, Mencl WE, et al. Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95(5):2636–41.CrossRef Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA, Pugh KR, Fulbright RK, Constable RT, Mencl WE, et al. Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95(5):2636–41.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Gentile M, Association AOT. Functional visual behavior in children: an occupational therapy guide to evaluation and treatment options: AOTA; 2005. Gentile M, Association AOT. Functional visual behavior in children: an occupational therapy guide to evaluation and treatment options: AOTA; 2005.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Slaghuis WL, Ryan JF. Spatio-temporal contrast sensitivity, coherent motion, and visible persistence in developmental dyslexia. Vision Res. 1999;39(3):651–68.CrossRef Slaghuis WL, Ryan JF. Spatio-temporal contrast sensitivity, coherent motion, and visible persistence in developmental dyslexia. Vision Res. 1999;39(3):651–68.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Training second-grade dyslexic students using a computerized program in Asyut, Egypt
verfasst von
W. M. Farghaly
S. H. El Tallawy
A. S. Ramadan
R. B. Abdelrasol
M. S. Omar
H. N. Eltallawy
K. O. Mohamed
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2022
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s41983-022-00480-y

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2022

The Egyptian Journal of Neurology, Psychiatry and Neurosurgery 1/2022 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Nicht Creutzfeldt Jakob, sondern Abführtee-Vergiftung

29.05.2024 Hyponatriämie Nachrichten

Eine ältere Frau trinkt regelmäßig Sennesblättertee gegen ihre Verstopfung. Der scheint plötzlich gut zu wirken. Auf Durchfall und Erbrechen folgt allerdings eine Hyponatriämie. Nach deren Korrektur kommt es plötzlich zu progredienten Kognitions- und Verhaltensstörungen.

Schutz der Synapsen bei Alzheimer

29.05.2024 Morbus Alzheimer Nachrichten

Mit einem Neurotrophin-Rezeptor-Modulator lässt sich möglicherweise eine bestehende Alzheimerdemenz etwas abschwächen: Erste Phase-2-Daten deuten auf einen verbesserten Synapsenschutz.

Sozialer Aufstieg verringert Demenzgefahr

24.05.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Ein hohes soziales Niveau ist mit die beste Versicherung gegen eine Demenz. Noch geringer ist das Demenzrisiko für Menschen, die sozial aufsteigen: Sie gewinnen fast zwei demenzfreie Lebensjahre. Umgekehrt steigt die Demenzgefahr beim sozialen Abstieg.

Hirnblutung unter DOAK und VKA ähnlich bedrohlich

17.05.2024 Direkte orale Antikoagulanzien Nachrichten

Kommt es zu einer nichttraumatischen Hirnblutung, spielt es keine große Rolle, ob die Betroffenen zuvor direkt wirksame orale Antikoagulanzien oder Marcumar bekommen haben: Die Prognose ist ähnlich schlecht.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.