Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Oral Health 1/2022

Open Access 01.12.2022 | Research article

Delay discounting, probability discounting, and interdental cleaning frequency

verfasst von: Anthony DeFulio, Mark Rzeszutek

Erschienen in: BMC Oral Health | Ausgabe 1/2022

Abstract

Background

Interdental cleaning is recommended by dentists but many people do not floss regularly. The health benefits of interdental cleaning are delayed, and sensitivity to delay is an important factor in many health behaviors. Thus, the present studies explore the relationship between frequency of flossing, and sensitivity to delayed and probabilistic outcomes.

Method

Crowd-sourced subjects were recruited in two studies (n = 584 and n = 321, respectively). In both studies, subjects reported their frequency of flossing and completed delay discounting and probability discounting tasks. Discounting was measured with area under the curve, and linear regression was used to analyze the results.

Results

Findings show that higher levels of delay discounting were associated with less frequent flossing (p < 0.001, both studies). In contrast, probability discounting was not significantly associated with flossing frequency (ns, both studies).

Conclusion

The findings are consistent with prior studies involving other health behaviors such as attendance at primary care and medication adherence. Results suggest that interventions that reduce delay discounting may help promote regular interdental cleaning, and that delay discounting is a more robust predictor of health behaviors than probability discounting. In addition, interdental cleaning appears to be a reasonable target behavior for evaluating potentially generalizable behavioral health interventions. Thus, interventions that are successful in promoting oral health behaviors should be considered as candidates for evaluation in other health behavior domains.
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s12903-022-02328-6.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
AUCord
Area under the curve with ordinal scaling

Background

The evidence in support of the oral health benefits of interdental cleaning (e.g., flossing) is limited [1, 2]. Nevertheless, dentists recommend it to their patients as part of a basic oral health care routine, and a public-facing webpage maintained by the American Dental Association characterizes interdental cleaning as an “essential part of taking care of your teeth and gums” (ADA website).
An estimated 32% of Americans report engaging in daily interdental cleaning [3]. For most people, the decision to floss is likely based on the recommendations of dental health professionals, or interactions with family, friends, or other trusted sources. This perspective is supported by the findings that the interdental cleaning behavior of mothers is the strongest predictor of the interdental cleaning behavior of their children [4], and that normative support is a key factor in the development of flossing automaticity [5]. It is also consistent with studies showing that flossing is positively correlated with expectations regarding its positive health impact [6]. Overall, belief that interdental cleaning is good appears to arise from social interactions and is common among people who regularly floss. Relatedly, a patient’s level of trust with their health care provider is associated with whether they follow medical advice [7].
Another important commonality among people who regularly floss is that they are confident they can do it properly, which is described as self-efficacy [8, 9]. Conversely, people who view flossing as an unpleasant activity are especially unlikely to do it [810]. Such barriers include concerns about bleeding gums and pain caused by flossing. Overall, the key predictors related to interdental cleaning are simple. The decision to integrate interdental cleaning into an oral health routine appears to be largely determined by a combination of an expectation it will be beneficial, confidence in the ability to do it, and a lack of barriers such as pain.
Many interventions have successfully targeted these factors as a means of increasing interdental cleaning (see [11], for a review). Although these interventions are informed by a variety of theoretical frameworks, they share similar features and collectively suggest that individualized assistance with planning is an especially effective intervention strategy. Integrating goal setting and self-monitoring into these individualized plans is also recommended [11].
Importantly, interdental cleaning shares a fundamental similarity to most other preventive health behaviors. Specifically, the health benefits are delayed instead of immediate, and are typically realized over timeframes from weeks to decades. The effects of these delays on health behavior have been studied extensively, resulting in the development of broadly applicable models of delay discounting [12, 13]. Individual differences in sensitivity to delayed outcomes is independent of other personality factors [14]. Integrating the study of probability discounting into studies of delay discounting enhances the value of the findings and involves a common analytical approach [15]. As such, investigation of the roles of delay and probability discounting in interdental cleaning may allow for further tailoring of interventions designed to promote interdental cleaning. In addition, comparison of interdental cleaning to other health behaviors may facilitate understanding of common behavioral processes that underlie preventive health behaviors. Thus, the purpose of the present study was to investigate associations between delay and probability discounting and self-reported frequency of interdental cleaning. Two experiments were conducted. As an exploratory measure, message framing was manipulated in the second experiment. Framing can affect attitudes, beliefs, and behavior regarding health decisions [16], as well as discounting [17]. Thus, inclusion of this variable in Experiment 2 allowed for additional points of comparison with the health behavior and decision-making literature.

Methods

Subjects

Subjects were workers recruited from Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk). MTurk is a crowdsourcing marketplace that allows for the electronic recruitment of convenience samples for academic research [18]. In both experiments, potential subjects were required to (1) have at least 100 approved human interface tasks (HITs), (2) have at least a 95% HIT approval rate, (3) be located the in US, and (4) not have already completed either experiment previously. This last requirement ensured that the experiments consisted of wholly unique samples. Based on Rzeszutek et al. [19] subjects were required to complete a short set of percentage comprehension questions as a screener in advance of taking the surveys. An incorrect answer on any screener question resulted in exclusion from both studies. The first page of the survey was a consent document that described the study and specified that continuation of participation served as demonstration of consent. In Experiment 1, compensation was $0.25 for completion of the screener and $3.00 for completion of the experimental survey. In Experiment 2, no compensation was provided for the screener, and $3.50 was paid for completion of the experimental survey. Sample size for the larger experiments from which these data were collected was determined based on an detecting a small to medium effect size (r = 0.2) for other covariates.

Procedures

Surveys were hosted by the online platform Qualtrics. In both experiments, demographic information and flossing were assessed in identical fashion prior to the discounting survey. All surveys used in the study are available as supplementary files. Flossing was assessed with a single multiple-choice question in which subjects selected how often they flossed. There were six options for flossing frequency: At least once a day, at least once a week, at least once a month, at least once in 6 months, at least once a year, and less than once a year. Subjects responded to the monetary discounting questions by moving sliders (i.e., dragging a button on a line to indicate a percentage or preference towards one of two outcomes). For each question, a smaller monetary amount (i.e., $500) was shown on the left and a larger monetary amount (i.e., $1000) was shown on the right. Probabilities were always expressed as percentages to subjects.
Figure 1 shows how the questions were presented, and complete instructions and experimental arrangements are provided separately (Additional files 1, 2). In Experiment 1, the smaller outcome was always set at the smallest delay (i.e., 1 day) and largest probability (i.e., 0.99). The larger outcome varied across questions in terms of probability and delay. Sets of five questions were presented in order of descending probability (0.99, 0.8, 0.5, 0.2, and 0.0). Within the set, delay to the larger outcome was held constant. Across sets, delay to the larger outcome was varied (1 day, 1 month, 6 months, 2 years, and 5 years). This resulted in 25 combinations of delays and probabilities of the larger outcome. Experiment 2 had similar questions, but instead of questions consisting of both delay and probability, outcomes were only delayed or probabilistic. The delays used were the same as in Experiment 1, except that 2 years was used as the largest delay instead of 5 years. Probabilities were the same as Experiment 1. Experiment 2 replicated Experiment 1, but differed in how probability and delay were manipulated. Experiment 2 also included an additional dichotomous independent variable; the framing of the values in terms of gain or loss. That is, some questions consisted of choices between smaller sooner/certain gains and larger later/uncertain gains, while others featured smaller sooner/certain losses and larger later/uncertain losses. This resulted in four different conditions, delayed gains, delayed losses, probabilistic gains, and probabilistic losses. Each condition was presented in a single block of five questions, with either ascending delays or descending probabilities, for a total of 20 questions.

Data analysis

Discounting was measured using area under the curve with ordinal scaling (AUCord; [20]). This was calculated with the formula AUCord = ∑i(1/nx)[(y1 + y2)/2], where nx is the number of total intervals between data points, or the number of questions in a condition minus 1, and where y1 and y2 are the normalized responses of the two points that indicate an interval. Because subject responses were already in percentages, where 0% was sliding all the way the left, and 100% was all the way to the right, normalization was simply the response divided by 100 to produce a proportion. The area of the polygon is calculated for each interval and then summed up to create a total value between 0 and 1. Because each interval is equally spaced, area differences between contiguous data points in delays or probabilities are carry equal weight for purposes of analysis. Higher values indicate lower or shallower discounting, whereas lower values indicate higher or steeper discounting. That is, higher values indicate little decrease in subjective value between the outcomes over the independent variable, whereas lower values indicate a large decrease in subjective value between the two outcomes over the independent variable. Because the smaller option always remained on the left for the slider questions, questions from Experiment 2 that involved losses were converted by subtracting the response from 100. This is account for the decrease in subjective value of the loss so that area differences between gains and losses could be directly compared.
To determine the relationship between flossing and discounting, statistical analyses were conducted using R 4.03 [21]. Because flossing questions were asked in a categorical manner rather than in a continuous one (i.e., how often you floss from these seven options rather than indicate how many days a year you floss), flossing frequency was treated as levels of a factor and a linear regression was conducted to determine how discounting changes as frequency of flossing decreases. That is, each group mean was compared to each other’s to determine differences between them.1 Daily flossing was used as the reference in the linear regression as it was expected that those who flossed daily would have the highest AUCord for delay discounting and lowest for probability discounting (i.e., least impulsivity/risk propensity), and thus would serve as a logical group to base comparisons from.
Post-hoc comparisons between all groups were also conducted using the emmeans package [22] with Holm-Bonferroni corrections to account for multiple comparisons. To analyze data from Experiment 1, AUCord was only calculated for the delays at 0.99 probability and probability at a delay of 1 day. This was to isolate the effects of delay relatively independent of probability and vice versa. To help ensure data quality, a slider question meant to determine attention of “Would you rather have $1000 immediately or $1 in a year?” was included in the survey. Values greater than 5% were excluded for purposes of analysis.

Results

Experiment 1

Demographic information is shown in Table 1. The sample (n = 584) was predominantly male (61.13%) and Caucasian (77.57%). The most commonly selected flossing frequency was flossing at least once a day (37.16%).
Table 1
Experiment 1 demographics and flossing
 
n/mean
%/SD
Age
37.96
± 19.33
Income
51,468.44
± 41,623
Sex
  
 Female
222
38.01%
 Male
357
61.13%
 Other
5
0.86%
Ethnicity
  
 Asian
46
7.88%
 Black/African
47
8.05%
 Caucasian
453
77.57%
 Hispanic/Latin
31
5.31%
 Other
7
1.20%
Flossing frequency
  
 At least once a day
217
37.16%
 At least once a week
185
31.68%
 At least once a month
75
12.84%
 At least once in 6 months
39
6.68%
 At least once a year
21
3.60%
 Less than once a year
47
8.05%
For discrete variables, the number of subjects and percentage of sample are included. For continuous variables, mean and SD are included
Discounting for delayed gains was related to self-reported flossing, F(5) = 7.502, p < 0.001. Regression outputs for both delay and probability discounting can be found in Table 2. Results of the regression of discounting of delayed gains indicated that there was a decrease in AUCord (i.e., higher preference for the smaller, sooner monetary gain) between daily flossing and less frequent flossing. That is, AUCord for delayed gains was higher for daily flossing relative to monthly, biannual, and yearly flossing. However, there was no significant difference between those who reported flossing daily, weekly, or less than once a year. By contrast, there was no relationship between flossing and probability discounting, F(5) = 0.891, p = 0.487. As shown in Table 1, group n tended to decrease as flossing frequency decreased. Figure 2 shows boxplots of delay and probability discounting as a function of self-reported flossing frequency. Post-hoc comparisons between all groups can be found in Table 3. Post-hoc comparisons generally agreed with the differences from daily flossing and other groups, although after corrections for multiple comparisons the difference between daily flossing and monthly flossing was no longer significant, t(578) = 2.46, p = 0.114.
Table 2
Experiment 1 regression of discounting on flossing
 
Delayed gain
Probabilistic gain
Estimate
SE
p
Estimate
SE
p
(Intercept)
0.673***
0.018
< 0.001
0.335***
0.011
< 0.001
At least once a week
− 0.026
0.026
0.306
0.013
0.016
0.411
At least once a month
− 0.085*
0.035
0.014
− 0.021
0.028
0.333
At least once in 6 months
− 0.194***
0.045
< 0.001
− 0.017
0.028
0.538
At least once a year
− 0.259***
0.059
< 0.001
− 0.016
0.037
0.671
Less than once a year
− 0.012
0.042
0.773
− 0.028
0.026
0.279
Parameter estimates for delay/probability discounting of gains and frequency of flossing. The intercept is the mean of discounting at the group that responded as flossing at least daily. Daily flossing is the reference for other parameter estimates in so far that each estimate that is not the intercept represents a mean increase or decrease from the group that responded as flossing at least daily
*Estimate is significant from daily flossers at the 0.05 level
**Estimate is significant from daily flossers at the 0.01 level
***Estimate is significant from daily flossers at p < 0.001. SE: standard error. Note that the intercept being significant simply refers to AUCord for daily flossing being significantly different from zero
Table 3
Post-hoc comparisons between flossing frequencies and delay/probability discounting
Contrast
Delayed gains
Probabilistic gains
Estimate
SE
df
t
p
Estimate
SE
df
t
p
Da–We
0.027
0.026
578
1.024
1
− 0.013
0.016
578
− 0.823
1
Da–Mo
0.085
0.035
578
2.46
0.114
0.021
0.022
578
0.968
1
Da–BiAn
0.194*
0.045
578
4.327
< 0.001
0.017
0.028
578
0.617
1
Da–An
0.259*
0.059
578
4.386
< 0.001
0.016
0.037
578
0.425
1
Da–< An
0.012
0.042
578
0.288
1
0.028
0.026
578
1.083
1
We–Mo
0.059
0.035
578
1.658
0.587
0.034
0.022
578
1.549
1
We–BiAn
0.168*
0.046
578
3.69
0.003
0.031
0.029
578
1.076
1
We–An
0.233*
0.06
578
3.908
0.001
0.029
0.037
578
0.78
1
We–< An
− 0.015
0.042
578
− 0.343
1
0.042
0.027
578
1.571
1
Mo–BiAn
0.109
0.051
578
2.143
0.228
− 0.004
0.032
578
− 0.114
1
Mo–An
0.174
0.064
578
2.726
0.06
− 0.005
0.04
578
− 0.131
1
Mo–< An
− 0.073
0.048
578
− 1.522
0.643
0.007
0.03
578
0.24
1
BiAn–An
0.065
0.07
578
0.923
1
− 0.002
0.044
578
− 0.037
1
BiAn–< An
− 0.182*
0.056
578
− 3.26
0.012
0.011
0.035
578
0.31
1
An–< An
− 0.247*
0.068
578
− 3.642
0.003
0.012
0.043
578
0.294
1
Contrasts of all conditions based on the flossing/discounting models in Experiment 1. Multiple comparisons were corrected using the Holm–Bonferroni method. Self-reported flossing frequency; Da: At least once a day. We: At least once a week. Mo: At least once a month. BiAn: At least once in 6 months. An: At least once a year. < An: Less than once a year
*Statistically significant differences from mean AUCord by flossing frequency at α = .05. Contrast: groups being compared. Estimate: estimated difference between flossing frequencies. SE: standard error. df: degrees of freedom

Experiment 2

Demographic information is shown in Table 4. As in Experiment 1, the sample (n = 321) was predominantly male (61.06%) and Caucasian (76.95%), and the most commonly selected flossing frequency was flossing at least once a day (33.96%). The proportions of subjects falling into each flossing frequency category were similar to those observed in Experiment 1.
Table 4
Experiment 2 demographics and flossing
 
n/mean
%/SD
Age
37.96
± 19.33
Income
51,612.71
± 47,083
Sex
  
 Female
124
38.63%
 Male
196
61.06%
 Other
1
0.31%
Ethnicity
  
 Asian
21
6.54%
 Black/African
27
8.41%
 Caucasian
247
76.95%
 Hispanic/Latin
23
7.17%
 Other
3
0.93%
Flossing frequency
  
 At least once a day
109
33.96%
 At least once a week
85
26.48%
 At least once a month
38
11.84%
 At least once in 6 months
35
10.90%
 At least once a year
27
8.41%
 Less than once a year
27
8.41%
For discrete variables, the number of subjects and percentage of sample are included. For continuous variables, mean and SD are included
Table 5 shows regression outputs of delay and probability discounting of gains and losses. Similar to Experiment 1, discounting of delayed gains was significant between self-reported flossing frequency, F(5) = 5.650, p < 0.001, but not for discounting of probabilistic gains, F(5) = 1.413, p = 0.219. For delay discounting, significant differences were observed for the same comparisons as in Experiment 1. Also similar to Experiment 1, probability discounting did not appear to be related to reported flossing frequency. Post-hoc comparisons were congruent with the regression analysis between daily flossing and other flossing frequencies. All post-hoc comparisons between flossing frequencies and discounting type can be found in Table 6.
Table 5
Experiment 2 regression of discounting on flossing
 
Delayed gain
Probabilistic gain
Estimate
SE
p
Estimate
SE
p
(Intercept)
0.692***
0.029
< 0.001
0.354***
0.018
< 0.001
At least once a week
− 0.042
0.044
0.339
− 0.000
0.027
0.994
At least once a month
− 0.218***
0.057
< 0.001
− 0.056
0.035
0.114
At least once in 6 months
− 0.219***
0.059
< 0.001
− 0.045
0.036
0.220
At least once a year
− 0.194**
0.064
0.003
− 0.058
0.040
0.147
Less than once a year
− 0.074
0.065
0.260
0.032
0.040
0.426
 
Delayed loss
Probabilistic loss
(Intercept)
0.814***
0.030
< 0.001
0.536***
0.020
< 0.001
At least once a week
− 0.068
0.045
0.128
0.024
0.030
0.427
At least once a month
− 0.192*
0.059
0.012
− 0.058
0.039
0.135
At least once in 6 months
− 0.202***
0.060
< 0.001
− 0.062
0.040
0.125
At least once a year
− 0.085
0.066
0.197
0.059
0.044
0.176
Less than once a year
− 0.100
0.067
0.135
0.074
0.044
0.097
Parameter estimates for delay/probability discounting of gains and frequency of flossing. Discounting of gains are in top half of the table, discounting of losses is in the bottom path of the table. The intercept is the mean of discounting at the group that responded as flossing at least daily. Daily flossing is the reference for other parameter estimates in so far that each estimate that is not the intercept represents a mean increase or decrease from the group that responded as flossing at least daily
*Estimate is significant from daily flossers at the 0.05 level
**Estimate is significant from daily flossers at the 0.01 level
***Estimate is significant from daily flossers at p < 0.001. SE: Standard error. Note that the intercept being significant simply refers to AUCord for daily flossing being significantly different from zero
Table 6
Post-hoc comparisons between flossing frequencies and delay/probability discounting
Contrast
Delayed gain
Probabilistic gain
Estimate
SE
df
t
p
Estimate
SE
df
t
p
Da–We
0.042
0.044
318
0.958
1
0
0.027
318
0.008
1
Da–Mo
0.218*
0.057
318
3.819
0.002
0.056
0.035
318
1.583
1
Da–BiAn
0.219*
0.059
318
3.729
0.003
0.045
0.036
318
1.228
1
Da–An
0.194*
0.064
318
3.031
0.034
0.058
0.04
318
1.455
1
Da–< An
0.074
0.065
318
1.13
1
− 0.032
0.04
318
− 0.798
1
We–Mo
0.176*
0.059
318
2.981
0.037
0.055
0.036
318
1.523
1
We–BiAn
0.177*
0.061
318
2.921
0.041
0.044
0.037
318
1.184
1
We–An
0.153
0.066
318
2.315
0.212
0.057
0.041
318
1.41
1
We–< An
0.032
0.067
318
0.475
1
− 0.032
0.041
318
− 0.782
1
Mo–BiAn
0.002
0.071
318
0.022
1
− 0.011
0.044
318
− 0.254
1
Mo–An
− 0.023
0.076
318
− 0.309
1
0.002
0.047
318
0.041
1
Mo–< An
− 0.144
0.076
318
− 1.891
0.536
− 0.088
0.047
318
− 1.864
0.949
BiAn–An
− 0.025
0.077
318
− 0.323
1
0.013
0.047
318
0.274
1
BiAn–< An
− 0.146
0.078
318
− 1.878
0.536
− 0.077
0.048
318
− 1.6
1
An–< An
− 0.121
0.082
318
− 1.48
0.98
− 0.089
0.05
318
− 1.777
1
 
Delayed loss
Probabilistic loss
Da–We
0.068
0.045
318
1.525
1
− 0.024
0.03
318
− 0.796
1
Da–Mo
0.192*
0.059
318
3.278
0.016
0.058
0.039
318
1.499
1
Da–BiAn
0.202*
0.061
318
3.346
0.014
0.062
0.04
318
1.536
1
Da–An
0.085
0.066
318
1.293
1
− 0.059
0.044
318
− 1.356
1
Da–< An
0.1
0.067
318
1.499
1
− 0.074
0.044
318
− 1.666
0.871
We–Mo
0.124
0.061
318
2.04
0.507
0.082
0.04
318
2.036
0.444
We–BiAn
0.134
0.063
318
2.144
0.427
0.085
0.042
318
2.059
0.444
We–An
0.017
0.068
318
0.249
1
− 0.036
0.045
318
− 0.792
1
We–< An
0.032
0.069
318
0.464
1
− 0.05
0.046
318
− 1.103
1
Mo–BiAn
0.01
0.073
318
0.139
1
0.003
0.049
318
0.069
1
Mo–An
− 0.107
0.078
318
− 1.378
1
− 0.118
0.052
318
− 2.284
0.282
Mo–< An
− 0.092
0.079
318
− 1.172
1
− 0.132
0.052
318
− 2.542
0.164
BiAn–An
− 0.117
0.079
318
− 1.481
1
− 0.121
0.053
318
− 2.308
0.282
BiAn–< An
− 0.102
0.08
318
− 1.279
1
− 0.136
0.053
318
− 2.561
0.164
An–< An
0.015
0.084
318
0.178
1
− 0.015
0.056
318
− 0.263
1
Contrasts of all conditions based on the flossing/discounting models in Experiment 2. Multiple comparisons were corrected using the Holm–Bonferroni method. Discounting of gains is on the top half of the table, whereas discounting of losses in on the bottom half of the table. Self-reported flossing frequency; Da: At least once a day. We: At least once a week. Mo: At least once a month. BiAn: At least once in 6 months. An: At least once a year. < An: Less than once a year
*Statistically significant differences from mean AUCord by flossing frequency at α = 0.05. Contrast: groups being compared. Estimate: estimated difference between flossing frequencies. SE: standard error. df: degrees of freedom
There was a significant relationship for discounting of delayed, F(5) = 3.561, p = 0.004, and probabilistic, F(5) = 2.580, p = 0.026, losses by flossing frequency. Nevertheless, there were no significant differences related to daily flossing and other flossing frequencies based on AUCord of probabilistic losses determined by the regression output, nor were there any significant post-hoc differences between flossing frequency and discounting of probabilistic losses after correction for multiple comparisons. Discounting of delayed losses was similar to delayed gains, with the exception that of annual flossing was not significantly different from daily flossing. Boxplots of delay and probability discounting for gains and losses by flossing frequency can be found in Fig. 3. Reported flossing also followed the same pattern as Experiment 1, with the sizes of groups decreasing as frequency of flossing decreased.

Discussion

Experiments 1 and 2 show that people who engage in daily interdental cleaning discount delayed gains less than people who engage in interdental cleaning occasionally throughout a year. This finding is consistent with prior studies of the relationship between delay discounting and other preventive health behaviors, including attending primary care visits [14, 23], medication adherence [24], and social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic [25]. It is also consistent with the preponderance of work in delay discounting and health behavior, which shows that greater discounting of delayed gains is associated with behavioral health problems such as substance use disorders and gambling [26].
In contrast, the present studies did not find support for a relationship between probabilistic discounting and frequency of interdental cleaning. This too, is consistent with weaker support for a relationship between probability discounting and cocaine use disorder [27], smoking [28], and obesity [29, 30]. Taken together, these results suggest that probability discounting is less robust as a predictor of health behavior. Further, the concordance in results across qualitatively different health behaviors suggests common processes underly human decision-making that affects health [12, 13].
One implication of the present finding involves interventions that were developed to improve health behavior by affecting discounting processes. Such interventions may be useful for improving interdental cleaning regularity, even if they were originally developed to address a different behavioral health problem. One especially promising intervention of this kind is episodic future thinking, in which a person imagines future autobiographical events [31]. Laboratory studies have shown that episodic future thinking can decrease discounting of delayed rewards [32, 33]. Although the evidence demonstrating that episodic future thinking can produce lasting change in health behavior is limited, small early studies show promising results in the domains of medication adherence [24] and smoking cessation [34]. An intervention in which patients are simply coached to practice vividly imagining a future in which they have good oral health because they flossed is seemingly practical. As such, it appears worthy of systematic evaluation. Another candidate intervention is exposure to nature. Viewing images of nature prior to completing a delay discounting task reduces discounting [35]. However, translational work in this nascent area is especially limited (see Berry et al. [36] for a brief review), and the manner in which this could be implemented in practice to increase the likelihood that people will include interdental cleaning in their oral health routine is not clear.
The present results also suggest that interdental cleaning could serve as an important target behavior for evaluating concepts and procedures that relate to health decisions and behaviors. It may seem that the population rate of flossing is low (32%; [3]). However, it is important to view this rate in light of the current and historical methods used to promote interdental cleaning. As described in the introduction, regular interdental cleaning is largely a product of recommendations, training, and rules provided by trusted dental professionals and close relatives. Any intervention that is developed for promoting interdental cleaning that better harness the available social supports should be considered a worthy candidate for generalization to other health behaviors. Further, fact that 32% of people floss regularly based on recommendations and rules could be viewed as a good outcome. More research on characteristics and practices of individuals who engage in interdental cleaning on a daily basis are worthy of investigation and also potentially applicable to other health behaviors. In this way, studies of interdental cleaning could be a source of novel and generalizable ideas that relate to the promotion of a wide variety of preventive health behaviors.
This study has several limitations, notably that it used crowd-sourced samples and that interdental cleaning regularity could not be verified. However, the similarity between the subject reports in the present study and those of Fleming et al. [3] adds confidence in our measure of interdental cleaning. Other potential problems with crowd-sourced samples were mitigated by other elements of the study design. This includes the initial comprehension check to be able to access the study, and the embedded attention check used to screen out bots or potentially inattentive responders (i.e., those trying to complete the survey as quickly as possible). In the present study, probability discounting, delay discounting, and self-reported interdental cleaning regularity were included in a battery of other experimental and demographic questions, which obscures potential influence by experimenter-hypothesized relationships. Confidence in the results is improved by the replication across two experiments with independent samples using different, but similar, discounting tasks. Nevertheless, independent replication of the relationship between discounting and interdental cleaning regularity is an important step for future research in this domain. Finally, the present study is silent with respect to causality between flossing and discounting, as well as whether other measures of impulsivity would show a similar relationship (e.g., [37]).

Conclusion

The present study expands the range of health behaviors relevant to discounting to include interdental cleaning. This expansion has relevance to the development of interventions designed to promote interdental cleaning, and suggests that preventive oral health practices offer a valuable context in which to generate and evaluate behavioral health interventions.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Declarations

The research described in this manuscript was reviewed and approved by the Western Michigan University Human Subjects Institutional Review Board (Protocol 20-04-09). All subjects provided electronic consent under a procedure approved by the Institutional Review Board.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Fußnoten
1
The F statistic from this type of linear regression (i.e., comparing categorical variables) is equivalent to the F statistic from an ANOVA.
 
Literatur
2.
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Lloyd A, McKay R, Hartman TK, Vincent BT, Murphy J, Gibson-Miller J, Levita L, Bennett K, McBride O, Martinez AP, Stocks TVA, Vallières F, Hyland P, Karatzias T, Butter S, Shevlin M, Bentall RP, Mason L. Delay discounting and under-valuing of recent information predict poorer adherence to social distancing measures during the COVID-19 pandemic. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):1–12. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-98772-5.CrossRef Lloyd A, McKay R, Hartman TK, Vincent BT, Murphy J, Gibson-Miller J, Levita L, Bennett K, McBride O, Martinez AP, Stocks TVA, Vallières F, Hyland P, Karatzias T, Butter S, Shevlin M, Bentall RP, Mason L. Delay discounting and under-valuing of recent information predict poorer adherence to social distancing measures during the COVID-19 pandemic. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):1–12. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1038/​s41598-021-98772-5.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Cox DJ, Dolan SB, Johnson P, Johnson MW. Delay and probability discounting in cocaine use disorder: comprehensive examination of money, cocaine, and health outcomes using gains and losses at multiple magnitudes. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2020;28(6):724–38. https://doi.org/10.1037/pha0000341.CrossRefPubMed Cox DJ, Dolan SB, Johnson P, Johnson MW. Delay and probability discounting in cocaine use disorder: comprehensive examination of money, cocaine, and health outcomes using gains and losses at multiple magnitudes. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2020;28(6):724–38. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​pha0000341.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Delay discounting, probability discounting, and interdental cleaning frequency
verfasst von
Anthony DeFulio
Mark Rzeszutek
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2022
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Oral Health / Ausgabe 1/2022
Elektronische ISSN: 1472-6831
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12903-022-02328-6

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2022

BMC Oral Health 1/2022 Zur Ausgabe

„Übersichtlicher Wegweiser“: Lauterbachs umstrittener Klinik-Atlas ist online

17.05.2024 Klinik aktuell Nachrichten

Sie sei „ethisch geboten“, meint Gesundheitsminister Karl Lauterbach: mehr Transparenz über die Qualität von Klinikbehandlungen. Um sie abzubilden, lässt er gegen den Widerstand vieler Länder einen virtuellen Klinik-Atlas freischalten.

Klinikreform soll zehntausende Menschenleben retten

15.05.2024 Klinik aktuell Nachrichten

Gesundheitsminister Lauterbach hat die vom Bundeskabinett beschlossene Klinikreform verteidigt. Kritik an den Plänen kommt vom Marburger Bund. Und in den Ländern wird über den Gang zum Vermittlungsausschuss spekuliert.

Darf man die Behandlung eines Neonazis ablehnen?

08.05.2024 Gesellschaft Nachrichten

In einer Leseranfrage in der Zeitschrift Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology möchte ein anonymer Dermatologe bzw. eine anonyme Dermatologin wissen, ob er oder sie einen Patienten behandeln muss, der eine rassistische Tätowierung trägt.

Ein Drittel der jungen Ärztinnen und Ärzte erwägt abzuwandern

07.05.2024 Klinik aktuell Nachrichten

Extreme Arbeitsverdichtung und kaum Supervision: Dr. Andrea Martini, Sprecherin des Bündnisses Junge Ärztinnen und Ärzte (BJÄ) über den Frust des ärztlichen Nachwuchses und die Vorteile des Rucksack-Modells.

Update Zahnmedizin

Bestellen Sie unseren kostenlosen Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert – ganz bequem per eMail.